Abstract

The exact anatomical localization of right hemisphere lesions that lead to left spatial neglect is still debated. The effect of confounding factors such as acute diaschisis and hypoperfusion, visual field defects, and lesion size may account for conflicting results that have been reported in the literature. Here, we present a comprehensive anatomical investigation of the gray- and white matter lesion correlates of left spatial neglect, which was run in a sample 58 patients with subacute or chronic vascular strokes in the territory of the right middle cerebral artery. Standard voxel-based correlates confirmed the role played by lesions in the posterior parietal cortex (supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus, and temporal–parietal junction), in the frontal cortex (frontal eye field, middle and inferior frontal gyrus), and in the underlying parietal–frontal white matter. Using a new diffusion tensor imaging-based atlas of the human brain, we were able to run, for the first time, a detailed analysis of the lesion involvement of subcortical white matter pathways. The results of this analysis revealed that, among the different pathways linking parietal with frontal areas, damage to the second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF II) was the best predictor of left spatial neglect. The group study also revealed a subsample of patients with neglect due to focal lesion in the lateral–dorsal portion of the thalamus, which connects the premotor cortex with the inferior parietal lobule. The relevance of fronto-parietal disconnection was further supported by complete in vivo tractography dissection of white matter pathways in 2 patients, one with and the other without signs of neglect. These 2 patients were studied both in the acute phase and 1 year after stroke and were perfectly matched for age, handedness, stroke onset, lesion size, and for cortical lesion involvement. Taken together, the results of the present study support the hypothesis that anatomical disconnections leading to a functional breakdown of parietal–frontal networks are an important pathophysiological factor leading to chronic left spatial neglect. Here, we propose that different loci of SLF disconnection on the rostro-caudal axis can also be associated with disconnection of short-range white matter pathways within the frontal or parietal areas. Such different local disconnection patterns can play a role in the important clinical variability of the neglect syndrome.

Introduction

In humans, spatial neglect is a debilitating neurological condition that is frequently associated with stroke in the right hemisphere (Gainotti et al. 1972; Oxbury et al. 1974; Weintraub and Mesulam 1987). Patients with neglect have reduced or no attention to events occurring in the contralesional side of extrapersonal (i.e. visual and auditory), personal (i.e. somatosensory), or representational (i.e. imagery space, Mesulam 1981) space and may fail to produce movements toward the unattended space (i.e. motor neglect). The cognitive resources of neglect patients are pathologically biased toward the ipsilesional right side of space, so that events taking place in the contralesional left side of space fail to receive full cognitive processing and to reach patients' awareness. Based on this acquired lack of awareness for one side of space, the study of the anatomical correlates of spatial neglect is considered an important source of evidence for the understanding of the anatomical foundation of consciousness and attention.

Since original seminal case reports (Brain 1941; Paterson and Zangwill 1944; Mcfie et al. 1950), spatial neglect has been classically interpreted as a “parietal sign” (Critchley 1953; Vallar and Perani 1986). However, ensuing investigations pointed out that spatial neglect can also arise from damage to frontal areas (Heilman and Valenstein 1972; Damasio et al. 1980; Mesulam 1981; Stein and Volpe 1983; Husain and Kennard 1996,1997; Husain et al. 2000) to the thalamus (Watson and Heilman 1979; Cambier et al. 1980; Watson et al. 1981; Graff-Radford et al. 1985; Hirose et al. 1985; Bogousslavsky et al. 1986; Motomura et al. 1986; Vallar and Perani 1986; Waxman et al. 1986; Rafal and Posner 1987; Kumral et al. 1995; Chung et al. 1996; Leibovitch et al. 1998; Karussis et al. 2000; Karnath et al. 2002; De Witte et al. 2008) and to subcortical gray matter nuclei. Due to the diffuse connections of subcortical gray matter nuclei with the cortical mantle, in this latter case, spatial neglect has been interpreted as originating from widespread reduction in the functioning of parietal and frontal areas connected with damaged subcortical nuclei. (Hier et al. 1977; Damasio et al. 1980; Healton et al. 1982; Stein and Volpe 1983; Ferro et al. 1987; Perani et al. 1987; Caplan et al. 1990; Weiller et al. 1990; Donnan et al. 1991; Weiller et al. 1993; Kumral et al. 1995; Chung et al. 1996; Leibovitch et al. 1998; Karnath et al. 2002).

More recently, anatomical (Leibovitch et al. 1998; Doricchi and Tomaiuolo 2003; Bartolomeo et al. 2007), neurosurgical (Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2005; Shinoura et al. 2009) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) investigations (He et al. 2007) have provided converging evidence, suggesting that disconnection of long-range white matter fiber bundles connecting parietal with frontal areas has a relevant role in engendering severe and chronic signs of spatial neglect both in humans and in monkeys (Gaffan and Hornak 1997). These findings provided new vigor to pioneering views by Critchley (1953), Geschwind (1965), and Mesulam (1981), who suggested that enduring lack of awareness for the left side of space can be influenced in a substantial way by damage to white matter connections in the right hemisphere.

One relevant and well-known clinical feature of the spatial neglect syndrome is its reduction in severity and incidence in the chronic when compared with the acute phase of the stroke. Although in the acute phase up to two-thirds of the right brain damaged patients can show spatial neglect signs, the majority of these patients show spontaneous recovery in the post-acute and chronic phase (Stone et al. 1992; Corbetta et al. 2005). In a recent report, using a cytoarchitectonic atlas based on postmortem staining of the myelin of coronal slices of the human brain (Bürgel et al. 2006), Karnath et al. (2009) concluded that damage of gray matter structures is a stronger predictor of acute spatial neglect than associative tract disconnection. However, these authors also commented that, in the acute phase, even small lesions can lead to temporary profound cerebral dysfunctions of large brain areas through mechanisms such as hypometabolism (Yasaka et al. 1998), hypoperfusion (Hillis et al. 2000), or acute diaschisis (Monakow 1914; Kempler et al. 1988; Price et al. 2001; Finger et al. 2004). Thus, the authors concluded that their study cannot decide whether or not spatial neglect should best be interpreted as a “disconnection syndrome” (Geschwind 1965; Watson et al. 1974; Mesulam and Geschwind 1978; Watson et al. 1978; Bartolomeo 2007; Doricchi et al. 2008) and further recommended conducting the same investigation with a large group of patients who have chronic injury.

A few case studies run with direct diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tracking of white matter pathways have already suggested that fronto-parietal (Shinoura et al. 2009; Ciaraffa et al. 2012) or fronto-occipital disconnection (Urbanski et al. 2008,2011) can be a strong predictor of spatial neglect. However, the role of white matter disconnection in chronic spatial neglect has never been formally tested in large group of patients by using adequate statistical procedures to map the lesion involvement of relevant white matter tracts. The aim of the present study was therefore to evaluate and contrast the respective contributions of gray- and white matter damage to chronic spatial neglect in an adequately large sample of right brain damaged patients.

Studies of the effect of brain lesions on behavior frequently assume that discrete anatomical modules handle specific cognitive functions (Rorden and Karnath 2004). Consequently, analyses to determine the behavioral implications of brain lesions are performed via statistical tests completed independently at each of the intersubjects aligned voxel (voxel-wise or topological statistics), without any attempt to capture correlations across them (Catani 2007; Husain and Nachev 2007). However, 2 voxels should be considered as correlated if they share common anatomical features. While 2 distant voxels cannot share the same neuronal soma, several voxels can pertain to the same axon. Therefore, while the architecture of the cortex and the subcortical nuclei is well suited for voxel-wise statistics, the study of white matter organization requires adapted statistics at the tract level (track-wise or hodological statistics; Catani and Mesulam 2008; Rudrauf et al. 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2008). Track-wise statistics require anatomical knowledge on how voxels in the white matter are associated in common pathways.

Several atlases make this anatomical information now available, but each of them has both advantages and disadvantages. Tracing studies in monkeys provide astonishing details of the white matter organization (Schmahmann and Pandya 2006); however, the precise identification of the homologous structures in human is not straightforward (Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2012). The human postmortem atlas (Bürgel et al. 2006), used by Karnath et al. (2009), offers high-quality resolution of white matter tracts running parallel to coronal slices used for myelin staining such as the cortico-spinal tract; however, as stressed by Bürgel et al. (2006), this technique strongly underestimates parietal–frontal association pathways that run orthogonally to coronal slices (Bürgel et al. 2006; Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011). Importantly, such tracts include some of the crucial pathways whose damage might be important to spatial neglect, such as the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) and the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF). To circumvent these limitations, in the present study, we used an atlas of white matter connections based on DTI dissection of the human brain. Despite some limitation in the resolution of tracts that cross between each other (Jones 2008; Dell'acqua et al. 2010), DTI atlases provide a representative description of the anatomy and variability of the association tracts in the human living brain, giving the opportunity to measure in vivo white matter pathways location, asymmetry and intersubject variability on large populations of patients (Catani et al. 2007; Lawes et al. 2008; Mori et al. 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011).

To investigate the role of gray- and white matter damage in spatial neglect, we used voxel-wise and track-wise regressions. Since it has been suggested that the chronicity and severity of spatial neglect can increase with the size of the brain damage (Levine et al. 1986; Perani et al. 1987), as well as with the presence of an associated visual field defect (Halligan et al. 1990; Doricchi and Angelelli, 1999; Doricchi et al. 2005), we covaried both of these factors out from the analyses.

To summarize, in a first step, we used a classical voxel lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) approach in order to allow a direct comparison with data previously gathered from samples of acute patients (Karnath et al. 2009). In a second step, we employed regression to reveal the location of the brain lesions and the pattern of disconnection that correlate with the occurrence of chronic spatial neglect; right deviation in line bisection and omissions in cancellation tasks covarying out the effect of the lesion size and the presence of an associated visual field defect. Gray matter correlates were studied using a voxel-wise approach that better fits the architecture of the cerebral cortex and the subcortical nuclei. White matter correlates were studied by using a track-wise statistical approach that takes into consideration how different voxels are associated along the same white matter pathway.

During the completion of the group study, we had the rather unique opportunity of performing a complete tractography dissection of white matter fiber tracts in 2 patients who were perfectly matched on all relevant clinical variables (i.e. lesion size, lesion location, stroke onset, sex, age, handedness, educational level), except for the presence or absence of spatial neglect both in the acute and chronic phase. As it will be shown in the following, direct and complete tractography dissection of these 2 cases added further evidence converging with the main results from the group study.

Materials and Methods

Group Study

Participants

Fifty-eight patients with a vascular stroke in the territory of the middle cerebral artery (MCA) in the right hemisphere participated in this study. Written and informed consent to participate to this research was obtained. The ethics committee of the Santa Lucia Hospital in Rome, Italy approved the study. Chronic visual field defects were assessed by Goldmann perimetry. Seven patients were found to have chronic visual field defects. Left spatial neglect was assessed with the line bisection test (5 trials, line length = 200 mm, cut-off score +6.5 mm; Azouvi et al. 2002) and the letter cancellation task (Diller et al. 1974; cut-off score left minus right omissions = >4; Pizzamiglio et al. 1989). All task stimuli were presented on a horizontally oriented A4 paper sheet. Thirty-eight patients were found to have varying degrees of left unilateral neglect (N+) on both tasks, whereas 20 patients were free from neglect signs (N−). The 2 groups were comparable for age (t(56) = 0.9, P = 0.3) and time of stroke onset (t(56) = 1.8, P = 0.18). As expected, N+ had a larger ipsilesional bias both in line bisection (t(56) = 5.1, P < 0.0001) and letter cancellation (t(56) = 7.6, P < 0.0001) than N−. On average, lesion size was higher in N+ when compared with N− (t(56) = 4, P < 0.001). Clinical and demographic data are reported in Table 1.

Table 1

Clinical and demographic data of the groups of patients with (N+) and without (N−) left spatial neglect

Age (years)Stroke onset (days)Line bisection (mm)Letter cancellation (omissions)Lesion size (cc)
N+ (n= 38)
 Mean61.11209.855.2144.1
 SD12.3113.77.129.5116.4
N− (n= 20)
 Mean57.670.41.13.434.8-
 SD13.759.33.1337.9
Age (years)Stroke onset (days)Line bisection (mm)Letter cancellation (omissions)Lesion size (cc)
N+ (n= 38)
 Mean61.11209.855.2144.1
 SD12.3113.77.129.5116.4
N− (n= 20)
 Mean57.670.41.13.434.8-
 SD13.759.33.1337.9

SD, standard deviations.

Table 1

Clinical and demographic data of the groups of patients with (N+) and without (N−) left spatial neglect

Age (years)Stroke onset (days)Line bisection (mm)Letter cancellation (omissions)Lesion size (cc)
N+ (n= 38)
 Mean61.11209.855.2144.1
 SD12.3113.77.129.5116.4
N− (n= 20)
 Mean57.670.41.13.434.8-
 SD13.759.33.1337.9
Age (years)Stroke onset (days)Line bisection (mm)Letter cancellation (omissions)Lesion size (cc)
N+ (n= 38)
 Mean61.11209.855.2144.1
 SD12.3113.77.129.5116.4
N− (n= 20)
 Mean57.670.41.13.434.8-
 SD13.759.33.1337.9

SD, standard deviations.

Individual Lesion Mapping

Ten of the 38 N+ patients and 5 of the 20 N− patients had CT scan examination. The remaining patients underwent radiological 1.5-T MRI examination, including T1, T2, fluid attenuated inversion recovery, and diffusion images. Radiological examination was run within 10 days from the time of neuropsychological testing. Mapping of lesions was performed using DISPLAY (http://packages.bic.mni.mcgill.ca). For each patient, we rotated the MNI template (Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI) from the MNI space to the orientation of the patients individual clinical MRI or CT slices scan. The lesion was then drawn on the reoriented template by an expert neuroanatomist and subsequently taken back to the MNI space using the inverse rotation (Doricchi and Tomaiuolo 2003).

General Voxel Lesion Symptom Mapping Analysis (Classic Approach)

In a first step, we used a classical VLSM approach in order to allow a direct comparison with data previously gathered from samples of acute patients (Karnath et al. 2009). We used MRIcron (www.sph.sc.edu/comd/rorden/mricron) to compute a Brunner Munzel test between the lesions of the spatial neglect group and the group of patient without spatial neglect for each voxel of the brain (Rorden et al. 2007). The results are corrected for multiple comparisons with a false discovery rate threshold (Genovese et al. 2002) and projected on a high-resolution template (Holmes et al. 1998) in the MNI using MRIcron (www.sph.sc.edu/comd/rorden/mricron).

Voxel-wise “Topological” Lesion-Deficit Analysis

We used MRIcron to compute regressions (Karnath et al. 2004; Rorden and Karnath 2004) in order to identify cortical areas that predicted whether a patient belonged or not to the N+ group (i.e. spatial neglect present or absent on both line bisection and letter cancellation tasks: A binary measure), cortical areas that predicted the degree of ipsilesional-rightward deviation in the line bisection task (a continuous measure of spatial neglect severity), and areas that predicted the number of omissions in the letter cancellation task (a continuous measure of spatial neglect severity). This approach identifies predictive regions by covarying out confounding factors such as, for example, the lesion size or the presence of chronic visual field defect. Time from stroke onset, that was not statistically different between N+ and N−, though numerically longer by 50 days in the N+ group (Table 1), did not need to be considered as a covariate in the analysis. This is because: 1) using stroke onset as a covariate would reveal the anatomical correlates of neglect independently of chronicity, contrary to the aims of our study; 2) differences in stroke onset time cannot account for our results, because it is logical to assume that N− patients, who did not show signs of neglect at an average of 70 days from onset, would not have turned into N+ patients at a longer time interval.

A first logistic regression used 3 independent variables: Lesion volume (a continuous measure), the presence or absence of visual field defects (a binary measure), and whether or not each single voxel (1 × 1 × mm) was damaged in each patient (a binary measure). We then calculated whether these 3 variables were able to predict the presence of chronic spatial neglect (the dependent variable and a binary measure).

A second linear regression used 3 independent variables: Lesion volume, the presence or absence of chronic visual field defects and whether or not the target voxel was damaged in each individual. We then calculated whether these 3 variables were able to predict the degree of ipsilesional-rightward deviation in line bisection (a continuous measure).

For the cancellation task, we used the same approach to identify cortical areas that have a predictive value for the number of omissions in the letter cancellation task (a continuous measure).

Track-wise “Hodological” Lesion-Deficit Analysis

We used a recently published DTI atlas (Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011) to describe the pattern of disconnection induced by each lesion at the individual level. The atlas provides a probability in the MNI for each voxel belonging to a specific track. When a patient's lesion overlapped on a voxel with a probability superior to 50% to contain a given tract (above the chance level), we considered this tract to be disconnected. We used SPSS software (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, United States of America) to compute regressions to identify the tracks whose lesion had a predictive value, after excluding confounding factors such as the presence of chronic visual field defects and the lesion size. Stroke onset was not considered as a covariate.

A first logistic regression used 3 independent variables: Lesion volume (a continuous measure), the presence or absence of chronic visual field defects (a binary measure), and whether or not the target tract was damaged in each patient (a binary measure). We then calculated whether these 3 variables were able to predict the presence of chronic spatial neglect (the dependent variable and a binary measure).

As a control analysis, we performed a second logistic regression by using the presence or absence of spatial neglect, lesion size, and whether or not the target tract was damaged in each individual as independent variables to calculate whether these variables were able to predict the presence of chronic visual field defects.

We then used linear regression to identify the tracks that had a predictive value for right deviation in line bisection and omissions in letter cancellation, after excluding confounding factors such as the presence of chronic visual field defects and the lesion size.

All track-wise hodological lesion-deficit results were subjected to Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (α level, P = 0.004).

Role of the Different SLF Branches in Spatial Neglect

We took advantage of a recently published atlas of the 3 branches of the SLF I, II, III (Thiebaut de Schotten, Dell'Acqua, et al. 2011) to report the pattern of disconnection induced by each lesion at the individual level by following the same track-wise hodological lesion-deficit described above. Results are presented after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison (α level, P = 0.0167).

Single-Case Study

We had the opportunity to run standard and advanced DTI tractography in 2 patients who were perfectly matched on all clinical relevant variables, except for the presence or absence of spatial neglect signs both in the acute and chronic phase of their cerebrovascular stroke (detailed clinical data are reported in Table 2).

Table 2

Study of the 2 single cases. Demographic and clinical data

N +
N-
SexMM
Age (years)5556
HandednessRR
Educational level (years)1313
Lesion size130.92145.48
Time Testing post-stroke20 days13 months20 days12 months
Line CancellationL 2/11 - R 10/10L 5/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10
Letter CancellationL 0/53 - R 40/51L 0/53 - R 41/51L 53/53 - R 49/51L 53/53 - R 49/51
N +
N-
SexMM
Age (years)5556
HandednessRR
Educational level (years)1313
Lesion size130.92145.48
Time Testing post-stroke20 days13 months20 days12 months
Line CancellationL 2/11 - R 10/10L 5/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10
Letter CancellationL 0/53 - R 40/51L 0/53 - R 41/51L 53/53 - R 49/51L 53/53 - R 49/51
Table 2

Study of the 2 single cases. Demographic and clinical data

N +
N-
SexMM
Age (years)5556
HandednessRR
Educational level (years)1313
Lesion size130.92145.48
Time Testing post-stroke20 days13 months20 days12 months
Line CancellationL 2/11 - R 10/10L 5/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10
Letter CancellationL 0/53 - R 40/51L 0/53 - R 41/51L 53/53 - R 49/51L 53/53 - R 49/51
N +
N-
SexMM
Age (years)5556
HandednessRR
Educational level (years)1313
Lesion size130.92145.48
Time Testing post-stroke20 days13 months20 days12 months
Line CancellationL 2/11 - R 10/10L 5/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10L 11/11 - R 10/10
Letter CancellationL 0/53 - R 40/51L 0/53 - R 41/51L 53/53 - R 49/51L 53/53 - R 49/51

Standard Diffusion Tensor Tractography

We used a 3-T whole-body system (Siemens Allegra) to acquire 60 diffusion-weighted volume directions and height volumes with no diffusion gradient applied covering the whole head of the patient with an isotropic resolution of 2 mm3. At each slice raw diffusion-weighted data were simultaneously registered and corrected for subject motion and geometrical distortions using ExploreDTI (http://www.exploredti.com; Leemans and Jones 2009). The tensor model was fitted to the data using the Levenberg–Marquardt nonlinear regression (Marquardt 1963). The fractional anisotropy (FA) was estimated in each voxel by scaling the water diffusion orientation from zero (random diffusion) to one (one direction only; Basser and Pierpaoli 1996). Whole-brain tractography was performed using an interpolated streamline algorithm that propagates from voxel to voxel following a step length of 0.5 mm and a maximum angle threshold of 35°. Voxel showing an FA value inferior to 0.2 was excluded from the tractography (Jones et al. 2002; Jones 2003, 2004). The whole-brain tractography was imported to “TrackVis” (Wedeen et al. 2008; http://www.trackvis.org) using a home-made software written in Matlab 2009b (http://www.matworks.com). Region of interest (ROIs) were defined on the axial FA images and were used as starting regions for tracking. Unlike other methods that use cortical masks as starting regions, the approach adopted here defines ROIs around the areas of white matter that represent “obligatory passages” along the course of each tract. Hence, the use of obligatory passages as starting seed points for tracking allows to visualize all fibers of a single tract without constraining its cortical projections, which may vary from one subject to the other. Using the ROIs previously described in Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten (2008), we performed single dissections of commissural pathways (anterior commissure and the corpus callosum), projection pathways (cortico-spinal, cortico-ponto-cerebellar, and the fornix) and associative pathways (fronto-temporal, fronto-parietal, and parieto-temporal segments of the arcuate, cingulum, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior occipito-frontal fasciculus, and uncinate fasciculus).

Advanced Spherical Deconvolution Tractography

Standard diffusion tensor tractography does not allow reconstructing the 3 branches of the SLF I, II and III (Thiebaut de Schotten, Dell'Acqua, et al. 2011) because of the crossing of the dorsal association fibers with commissural and projection fibers (Thiebaut de Schotten, Dell'Acqua, et al. 2011). Therefore, spherical deconvolution was chosen to estimate multiple orientations in voxels containing different populations of crossing fibers (Alexander 2006). A modified (damped) version of the Richardson-Lucy algorithm for spherical deconvolutions (Dell'acqua et al. 2010) was employed using the Software StarTrack (http://www.natbrainlab.com). Algorithm parameters were chosen as described before (Dell'Acqua et al. 2012). A fixed fiber response corresponding to a shape factor of α = 2 × 10–3 mm2/s was chosen (Dell'acqua et al. 2012). Fiber orientation estimates were obtained by selecting the orientation corresponding to the peaks (local maxima) of the fiber orientation distribution (FOD) profiles. To exclude spurious local maxima, we applied an absolute and a relative threshold. A first “absolute” threshold was used to exclude small local maxima due to noise or isotropic tissue. This threshold is 3 times the amplitude of a spherical FOD obtained from a gray matter isotropic voxel. A second “relative” threshold of 8% of the maximum amplitude of the FOD was applied to remove the remaining local maxima with values greater than the absolute threshold (Dell'Acqua et al. 2009). Tractography dissection of the SLF I, II and III was performed using a multiple ROIs approach as previously described in Thiebaut de Schotten, Dell'Acqua, et al. (2011).

Results

Group Study

General Voxel Lesion Symptom Mapping analysis

The Brunner Munzel test between the lesions of the spatial neglect group (N+) and the group of patient without signs of spatial neglect (N−) revealed a significant difference (P < 0.05) localized mainly in the perisylvian white matter, but also involving other areas such as the posterior part of the middle and the inferior frontal gyri, the pre- and postcentral gyri, the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ; traditionally including supramarginal, angular, and posterior part of the superior temporal gyri), and the white matter just below the frontal eye fields (Fig. 1).

Overlay of lesions in (a) patients without left spatial neglect (N−; n = 20); (b) patients with left spatial spatial neglect (N+; n = 38); (c) statistical analysis comparing the 2 populations of patients (N+ vs. N−; results are corrected for multiple comparisons, P < 0.05 for Z > 2.1). Ang = angular gyrus; FEF = frontal eye field; IFg = inferior frontal gyrus; MFg = middle frontal gyrus; Pog = postcentral gyrus; Prg = precentral gyrus; SMg = supramarginal gyrus; STg = superior temporal gyrus.
Figure 1.

Overlay of lesions in (a) patients without left spatial neglect (N−; n = 20); (b) patients with left spatial spatial neglect (N+; n = 38); (c) statistical analysis comparing the 2 populations of patients (N+ vs. N−; results are corrected for multiple comparisons, P < 0.05 for Z > 2.1). Ang = angular gyrus; FEF = frontal eye field; IFg = inferior frontal gyrus; MFg = middle frontal gyrus; Pog = postcentral gyrus; Prg = precentral gyrus; SMg = supramarginal gyrus; STg = superior temporal gyrus.

Voxel-wise Topological Lesion-Deficit Analysis

Previous voxel-wise investigations have rarely controlled for the effect of lesion size. A notable exception is the study by Karnath et al. (2004) who, however, showed that when lesion size is used as a regressor in the voxel-wise analysis, results do not survive correction for multiple comparisons. In line with this finding, also in our study, results lost most of their significance when lesion size was used as a covariate and correction for multiple comparisons was applied. To allow comparison with previous studies in the following, we report results not corrected for multiple comparisons. These anatomical findings were projected on a high-resolution template (Holmes et al. 1998) in the MNI by using MRIcron and are showed in Figure 2.

Voxel-wise (topological) lesion-deficit analysis. (a) MNI voxels predicting the presence versus absence of left spatial neglect (binary measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. (b) Damaged MNI voxels predicting the severity of ipsilesiona-rightward deviation in line bisection (continuous measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. (c) Damaged MNI voxels predicting omissions in the letter cancellation task (continuous measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. Results are not corrected for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05 for Z > 1.64). Ang = angular gyrus; FEF = frontal eye field; IFg = inferior frontal gyrus; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; MFg = middle frontal gyrus; Pog = postcentral gyrus; Prg = precentral gyrus; SMg = supra marginal gyrus; SPL = superior parietal lobule.
Figure 2.

Voxel-wise (topological) lesion-deficit analysis. (a) MNI voxels predicting the presence versus absence of left spatial neglect (binary measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. (b) Damaged MNI voxels predicting the severity of ipsilesiona-rightward deviation in line bisection (continuous measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. (c) Damaged MNI voxels predicting omissions in the letter cancellation task (continuous measure) when covarying out the presence of chronic visual field defect and lesion size. Results are not corrected for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05 for Z > 1.64). Ang = angular gyrus; FEF = frontal eye field; IFg = inferior frontal gyrus; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; MFg = middle frontal gyrus; Pog = postcentral gyrus; Prg = precentral gyrus; SMg = supra marginal gyrus; SPL = superior parietal lobule.

A logistic regression was performed to identify lesion loci crucial to the presence versus absence of spatial neglect (i.e. binary measure) when covarying out the lesion size and the presence of visual field defect. The results (Fig. 2a) revealed that especially lesions in the white matter are significant predictors of spatial neglect. However, other areas were also critical (Fig. 2a): The supramarginal gyrus and the adjacent most posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus at the temporal–parietal junction [this latter temporal area should not be confounded with the most anterior–central sector of the superior temporal gyrus advocated by Karnath et al. (2001, 2004, 2011)], the intraparietal sulcus adjacent to the angular gyrus, and the superior parietal lobe and a small lesion spot in the frontal eye fields.

In agreement with previous evidence (Binder et al. 1992; Verdon et al. 2010; Aiello et al. 2012), the separate linear regressions performed, respectively, on the continuous measures of rightwards deviations in line bisection and omissions in the letter cancellation task, provided slightly different results. For the line bisection task, there was a significant involvement of several areas in the white matter and cortical areas such as the precentral gyrus (“hand-knob” region), the supramarginal gyrus, the middle and inferior frontal gyri (partes opercularis and triangularis), the temporal–parietal junction, the intraparietal sulcus between the angular gyrus and the superior parietal lobule, and 2 small lesion spots, that is, one in the superior parietal lobule and another in the frontal eye fields (Fig. 2b). The linear regressions performed on the continuous measure of omissions in the letter cancellation task revealed no relevant involvement of parietal areas and a significant implication of the dorsal sectors of the middle frontal gyrus, the white matter just below the frontal eye field, the precentral gyrus (hand-knob region), the adjacent postcentral area, a lesion spot including the posterior portion of middle and inferior temporal gyri (extrastriate cortex), and the underlying white matter.

Track-Wise or Hodological Lesion-Deficit Analysis

Here, except when specifically indicated, we report results that survived the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. A logistic regression was performed to identify tracks crucial to left spatial neglect when covarying out the lesion size and the presence of chronic visual field defect. The analysis revealed that disconnection of the fronto-parietal segment of the arcuate fasciculus (β = 3.782, P = 0.002) was a significant predictor of spatial neglect (Fig. 3a). The analysis also disclosed an involvement of the fronto-temporal segment of the arcuate fasciculus (β = 2.4, P = 0.007), although this result did not survive to Bonferroni correction. As a control, we performed an identical analysis to identify tracks dedicated to chronic visual field defect, after covarying out the lesion size and the presence of spatial neglect. As anticipated, disconnection of the optic radiations resulted to be a significant predictor of chronic visual field defects (β = 3.031, P = 0.003; Fig. 3b).

Track-wise (hodotopic) lesion-deficit analysis. (a) Percentages of patients with and without left spatial neglect, with disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (b) Percentage of hemianopic patients and patients without visual field defects, with disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (c) The performance on the line bisection task (mean deviation with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (d) The performance on the letter cancellation task (mean omissions with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. *Tracts significantly involved when covarying out the presence of chronic neglect and the lesion size. Only results that survived Bonferroni correction are reported.
Figure 3.

Track-wise (hodotopic) lesion-deficit analysis. (a) Percentages of patients with and without left spatial neglect, with disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (b) Percentage of hemianopic patients and patients without visual field defects, with disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (c) The performance on the line bisection task (mean deviation with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. (d) The performance on the letter cancellation task (mean omissions with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the examined white matter tracts. *Tracts significantly involved when covarying out the presence of chronic neglect and the lesion size. Only results that survived Bonferroni correction are reported.

For line bisection, linear regression covarying out the lesion size and the presence of chronic visual field defects revealed that disconnection of the fronto-parietal segment of the arcuate fasciculus (β = 0.576, P = 0.00002) was a significant predictor for rightward deviations on line bisection (Fig. 3c).

For the cancellation task, multinomial regression, after covarying out lesion size, revealed that disconnection of the fronto-parietal segment of the arcuate fasciculus (β = 0.403, P = 0.001) was a significant predictor of omissions on the cancellation task (Fig. 3d).

The Role of Distinct SLF Branches in Spatial Neglect

A logistic regression analysis was run to investigate the specific involvement of each of the SLF branches (SLF I, II, and III) in spatial neglect (Fig. 4a). The initial results from this analysis showed that SLF II disconnection was the most likely lesion to predict chronic spatial neglect (β = 2.334, P = 0.004). SLF III disconnection also significantly resulted (β = 1.621, P = 0.023), but this result did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

SLF disconnection. (a) Percentage of patients with and without left spatial neglect with disconnection of SLF I, II, and III. (b) The performance on the line bisection task (mean deviation with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the 3 different branches of the SLF (I, II, and III). (c) The performance in the letter cancellation task (mean omissions with 95% confidence intervals) in patients with or without disconnection of each of the 3 different branches of the SLF (I, II, and III). Asterisks indicate SLF branches showing significant lesion involvement after covarying out the presence of visual field defect and lesion size. Only results that survived Bonferroni correction are presented.
Figure 4.

SLF disconnection. (a) Percentage of patients with and without left spatial neglect with disconnection of SLF I, II, and III. (b) The performance on the line bisection task (mean deviation with 95% confidence intervals) for patients with or without disconnection of each of the 3 different branches of the SLF (I, II, and III). (c) The performance in the letter cancellation task (mean omissions with 95% confidence intervals) in patients with or without disconnection of each of the 3 different branches of the SLF (I, II, and III). Asterisks indicate SLF branches showing significant lesion involvement after covarying out the presence of visual field defect and lesion size. Only results that survived Bonferroni correction are presented.

Linear regression analyses were run to investigate the possible specific involvement of each of the 3 SLF branches in the line bisection and letter cancellation tasks. For line bisection, multinomial regression when covarying out the lesion size and the presence of visual field defect, confirmed that disconnection of the SLF II (β = 0.536, P = 0.0002) was a significant predictor for rightwards deviations in line bisection (Fig. 4b). SLF III was also significantly reported (β = 0.314, P = 0.017), but this result did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. For the cancellation task, the multinomial regression revealed that disconnection of the SLF II (β = 0.349, P = 0.002) was a significant predictor of omissions, when covarying out the lesion size. SLF I was also significantly involved (β = 0.245, P = 0.02), but this result did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (Fig. 4c).

Lesion Localization in Spatial Neglect Patients without Parietal–Frontal Disconnection

Interestingly, 7 of the 38 patients showing chronic spatial neglect did not show neuroimaging signs of fronto-parietal disconnection. These patients had smaller lesions when compared with spatial neglect patients with parietal–frontal disconnection (t(36) = 3.421, P = 0.001). Six of these 7 patients had no concomitant visual field defect. There were no significant differences between this subgroup and the fronto-parietal one in performance on line bisection (t(36) = 1.205, P = 0.235) and cancellation tasks (t(36) < 1). In this small group, we performed a simple overlap of individual lesions (Bates et al. 2003). Based on a tractography atlas of connectivity-based segmentation of the thalamus (Behrens et al. 2003; FSL 4.1 software package; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl), we found that individual lesions overlapped within the latero-dorsal portion of the thalamus (Fig. 5). This part of the thalamus has strong connections with the premotor cortex (Behrens et al. 2003).

(a) Overlay of lesions in neglect patients without fronto-parietal disconnection. (b) Tractography atlas of the connectivity-based segmentation of the thalamus (Behrens et al. 2003).
Figure 5.

(a) Overlay of lesions in neglect patients without fronto-parietal disconnection. (b) Tractography atlas of the connectivity-based segmentation of the thalamus (Behrens et al. 2003).

Single-Case Study

The 2 patients considered in this study showed minor differences in the pattern of cortical damage, with the superior temporal and the inferior frontal gyri being partially preserved in the spatial neglect patient and almost entirely damaged in the non-neglect patient (Fig. 6). Lesion volume was slightly higher in the non-neglect patient (Table 2). Figure 7 shows the reconstruction of the posterior segment, the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and the cingulum for the 2 patients. Both patients had comparable partial disconnection of the corpus callosum and a complete disconnection of the IFOF and the uncinate fasciculus. The patient with spatial neglect had an additional disconnection of the fronto-parietal segment and the fronto-temporal segment of the arcuate fasciculus and disconnection of the cortico-ponto-cerebellar tract, which were instead normally reconstructed in the non-neglect patient. Advanced spherical tractography of the SLF I, II, and III revealed that both patient had preserved SLF I and damaged SLF III. SLF II was solely damaged in the spatial neglect patient (Fig. 8).

Study of the 2 single cases. (a) MRI-based reconstructions of the lesion for the patient with left spatial neglect (N+, top left) and for the patient without signs of neglect (N−, top right). The blue and red lines superimposed on the lateral views of the 3-dimensional reconstructions of the right hemisphere indicate the lateral (sylvian) sulcus and the superior temporal sulcus, respectively. The light blue and yellow arrows indicate the intraparietal sulcus and the vertical ramus of the sylvian fissure, respectively. (b) Axial sections showing the localization and extent of brain damage in the 2 patients. Red arrows indicate subcortical areas where the damage produced disconnection of parietal–frontal pathways in N+; green arrows indicate corresponding spared areas in N−. (c) Coronal sections passing at Y = −28 and Y = −32 (MNI coordinates) in N+ and N− patients. Red arrows indicate corresponding point of disconnection in coronal and transversal slices in N+ patient. On the rightmost column, the lesion of N+ is drawn in red and that of the N− patient in green. The overlap between the 2 lesions is shown in blue.
Figure 6.

Study of the 2 single cases. (a) MRI-based reconstructions of the lesion for the patient with left spatial neglect (N+, top left) and for the patient without signs of neglect (N−, top right). The blue and red lines superimposed on the lateral views of the 3-dimensional reconstructions of the right hemisphere indicate the lateral (sylvian) sulcus and the superior temporal sulcus, respectively. The light blue and yellow arrows indicate the intraparietal sulcus and the vertical ramus of the sylvian fissure, respectively. (b) Axial sections showing the localization and extent of brain damage in the 2 patients. Red arrows indicate subcortical areas where the damage produced disconnection of parietal–frontal pathways in N+; green arrows indicate corresponding spared areas in N−. (c) Coronal sections passing at Y = −28 and Y = −32 (MNI coordinates) in N+ and N− patients. Red arrows indicate corresponding point of disconnection in coronal and transversal slices in N+ patient. On the rightmost column, the lesion of N+ is drawn in red and that of the N− patient in green. The overlap between the 2 lesions is shown in blue.

Complete DTI dissection of white matter fiber tracts in the 2 matched cases with (N+) and without (N−) visuospatial neglect. Disconnected tracts were manually drawn in gray.
Figure 7.

Complete DTI dissection of white matter fiber tracts in the 2 matched cases with (N+) and without (N−) visuospatial neglect. Disconnected tracts were manually drawn in gray.

Spherical deconvolution dissection of white matter fiber tracts in the 2 matched cases with (N+) and without (N−) visuospatial neglect. Disconnected tracts were manually drawn in gray.
Figure 8.

Spherical deconvolution dissection of white matter fiber tracts in the 2 matched cases with (N+) and without (N−) visuospatial neglect. Disconnected tracts were manually drawn in gray.

Discussion

In this study, we used a recently published atlas of human brain connections (Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011; Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten 2012) to formally test, in a relatively large sample of the right brain damaged patients, whether lesion of white matter parietal–frontal pathways is a relevant anatomical determinant in the persistence of left spatial neglect in the chronic phase of cerebrovascular stroke. Three main findings emerged from our investigation. First, the most reliable predictor of chronic spatial neglect was the disconnection of one of the branches of the SLF, that is, the SLF II. Additional support to this group study result came by the complete white matter tractography dissection run in 2 clinically matched patients, one with and the other without signs spatial neglect. Although both of these patients had lesions of similar cortical location and size, only the patient with subcortical parietal–frontal disconnection showed signs of spatial neglect both in the acute and in the chronic phase. Secondly, the most reliable predictor of chronic spatial neglect in the gray matter was damage to the middle frontal gyrus and/or the TPJ (i.e. the ventral attentional system) although, in some cases, the lesioned area extended to or was close to areas belonging to the dorsal attentional system, such as the frontal eye field and the superior parietal lobule (Shomstein et al. 2010). Thirdly, in the small subgroup of chronic spatial neglect patients who did not have neuroimaging signs of parietal–frontal disconnection, most lesions affected the latero-dorsal portion of the thalamus.

Spatial Neglect as Both a “Disconnective Breakdown” and a “Disconnective Syndrome”

The role of fronto-parietal white matter disconnection confirms, at an extended group level, evidence gathered from neurosurgical studies in the monkey (Gaffan and Hornak 1997) and from more recent neurosurgical, fMRI, and DTI investigations run in relatively small samples of human patients (Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2005; He et al. 2007; Urbanski et al. 2008, 2011; Shinoura et al. 2009; Ciaraffa et al. 2012). Altogether, the present evidence supports the conclusions of previous lesion studies that, without beneficiating of the later development of tractography-based track-wise statistics, indicated a role of selective SLF disconnection (Doricchi and Tomaiuolo 2003) in human spatial neglect.

This converging evidence from different studies and methods suggests, rather homogenously, that cerebrovascular damage to long-range white matter parietal–frontal tracts is an important cause of spatial neglect. It is therefore important to discuss the possible pathophysiological mechanisms through which this type of lesion can engender chronic signs of spatial neglect in human patients. A neurological syndrome is usually interpreted as being purely disconnective, when cortical areas that get reciprocally disconnected by brain damage remain functionally and anatomically unaffected whereas any other function depending on their interaction is disrupted (Catani and Mesulam 2008). One might wonder whether this is the case for spatial neglect due to damage of parietal–frontal white matter pathways in the MCA territory. It has been suggested (Doricchi and Tomaiuolo 2003; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2005; Bartolomeo et al. 2007) that a lesion centered in the tightly packed parietal–frontal white matter fibers can be more disrupting than a cortical lesion of equivalent volume, because it can provoke widespread hypofunctioning of the whole network of interconnected cortical areas. In this sense, MCA spatial neglect could be better interpreted as a disconnective breakdown rather than a purely disconnective syndrome, that is, as resulting from dysfunction of large-scale cortical networks. Further converging evidence supporting this hypothesis comes from recent fMRI studies, demonstrating that the temporal correlation (i.e. functional connectivity) between the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signals in ventral parietal (TPJ) and frontal areas (IFG–MFG) is crucially disrupted in spatial neglect patients (He et al. 2007).

It is noteworthy, however, that there are other cases in which neglect can be properly interpreted as a purely disconnective syndrome. Lesions in the right posterior cerebral artery (PCA) territory that cause disconnection of the splenium of the corpus callosum and concomitant damage of the adjacent right striate cortex with accompanying left hemianopia provoke severe left side visual neglect both in humans and in monkeys (Gaffan and Hornak 1997; Bird et al. 2006; Park et al. 2006; Tomaiuolo et al. 2010). Based on a pioneering neurosurgical modeling study run in the monkey, Gaffan and Hornak (1997) have proposed that in this case visual neglect is severe because splenial disconnection confines the visual input arriving from the seeing right visual field in the left hemisphere. Thus, this information cannot reach the anatomically intact attentional parietal–frontal network in the right hemisphere. As a consequence, no exploration of the left unseen hemifield is possible. This pathophysiological mechanism mirrors in the right hemisphere the anatomical–functional impairment that in the left hemisphere produces “pure alexia” without agraphia, a prototypical example of pure disconnective syndrome (Dejerine 1891, 1892; Catani and ffytche 2005), where interruption of callosal fibers conveying visual inputs from the right hemisphere to language centers in the left hemisphere impairs reading with preserved visual and verbal functions. In the case of PCA neglect due to right splenial disconnection, the purely disconnective nature of the syndrome is further confirmed by the finding that in these patients neglect is restricted to the visual domain with no accompanying signs of personal, representational-imagery, or motor neglect (Tomaiuolo et al. 2010).

The Contribution of “Long-Range” and “Short-Range” Disconnections in MCA Spatial Neglect

There is shared agreement that spatial neglect is not a unitary syndrome made up of a homogenous collection of symptoms (Doricchi et al. 2008; Verdon et al. 2010). Spatial neglect cannot only selectively affect different sectors of space (extrapersonal vs. personal vs. representational space) or different spatial reference frames (egocentric vs. allocentric), but also different modes of spatial attentional processing as, for example, the simultaneous versus sequential analysis of spatial positions that are entailed by the line bisection and multiple item cancellation tasks, respectively (Binder et al. 1992). Based on this clinical evidence, recent research on the anatomical correlates of spatial neglect has moved from the study of the general anatomical correlates of the syndrome, toward the more focused study of the correlates of the specific clinical features characterizing different forms of the syndrome, or the different types of attentional disturbances associated with the spatial neglect syndrome (i.e. top-down vs. bottom up attention; Shomstein et al. 2010). Expanding on the original findings by Binder et al. (1992), Verdon et al. (2010) used the VLSM technique to demonstrate that different behavioral components of chronic spatial neglect are associated with lesion of different sectors of the right parietal–frontal attentional network. Consistent with original fMRI findings in healthy participants (Fink et al. 2000) and with the results from the present study, Verdon et al. (2010) identified a perceptual visuospatial component revealed by the performance on line bisection, text, and word reading tasks, and associated with lesions of the parietal lobe. By contrast, an exploratory visuomotor component revealed by the performance in sequential multi-item cancellations tasks and was associated with lesion of frontal areas. However, in each of these cases, lesion of the parietal and frontal components of the attentional network was associated with lesion involvement of the underlying white matter. This finding suggests that lesions encroaching upon long white matter connections do not only have diffuse disruptive effects on the entire right hemispheric network and that, depending on their relative posterior or anterior location (Doricchi et al. 2008), they may contribute to the selective functional disruption of more local circuits supported by short-range white matter fibers within the parietal or the frontal lobe (Fig. 9). In keeping with this hypothesis, spatial neglect patients with lesions centered on the supramarginal gyrus show significant changes in task-related BOLD activity in the adjacent visual cortex (Corbetta et al. 2005), whereas those with lesions centered in the inferior frontal gyrus display a breakdown of functional connectivity in more dorsal frontal areas (He et al. 2007). Note, however, that neither lesions limited to the associative visual cortex, nor those restricted to the dorsal frontal regions, typically produce spatial neglect. In summary, spatial neglect patients might demonstrate more perceptual or visuomotor symptoms depending on loss of function induced, by local anatomical diaschisis or functional breakdown, in the cortical areas surrounding the white matter lesion (Fig. 9). This conclusion received support by the recent finding (Aiello et al. 2012) that the severity of spatial neglect in a multiple item letter cancellation task correlates with frontal white matter damage producing both disconnection of the parietal–frontal pathways (i.e. SLF and arcuate fasciculus) and disconnection of a local white matter pathway linking the supplementary motor area and the superior frontal gyrus with the inferior frontal gyrus (Lawes et al. 2008; Oishi et al. 2008; Catani et al. 2012; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2012).

A diaschisis model of the symptomatological dissociation observed between left spatial neglect deriving from parietal or frontal strokes. (a) Parietal and (b) frontal strokes interrupting long-range parietal–frontal connections, and producing general hypoactivation of the parietal–frontal attentional network, can be associated with interruption of short-range connections causing local dysfunction of the neighboring areas in the parietal or in the frontal lobe.
Figure 9.

A diaschisis model of the symptomatological dissociation observed between left spatial neglect deriving from parietal or frontal strokes. (a) Parietal and (b) frontal strokes interrupting long-range parietal–frontal connections, and producing general hypoactivation of the parietal–frontal attentional network, can be associated with interruption of short-range connections causing local dysfunction of the neighboring areas in the parietal or in the frontal lobe.

It is worth of note that this pathophysiological mechanism can produce chronic effects having the same behavioral selectivity of transitory effects described by Hillis et al. (2000) in acute spatial neglect patients (Khurshid et al. 2012). Using diffusion- and perfusion-weighted MRI, these authors showed that, in the hyperacute phase, neglect limited to specific spatial coordinates (egocentric vs. object-centered neglect) is associated with local hypoperfusion of specific cortical areas, caused by temporary stenosis of blood vessels. Neglect disappeared when pharmacological intervention restored proper blood perfusion.

Gray Matter Components of Spatial Neglect

The voxel-based analyses run in the present study, when taking into account the lesion size as a regressor did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. This result, similarly to preliminary evidences reported by Karnath et al. (2004), confirm that lesion-deficit analyses focused on the severity spatial neglect are frequently biased by the effect of the lesion size. This result also strongly supports the idea of a distributed cortical origin for spatial neglect underpowering standard voxel-wise topological lesion-deficit analysis. Nevertheless, uncorrected analyses systematically showed that gray matter lesions predicting signs of chronic spatial neglect concern regions reciprocally interconnected through the arcuate fasciculus and SLF, such as the TPJ and the middle frontal gyrus (Catani et al. 2005, 2007; Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2008; Tsang et al. 2009). This finding is consistent with earlier observations that were focused on the role of localized gray matter damage in spatial neglect (Vallar and Perani 1986; Husain and Kennard 1996, 1997; Mort et al. 2003; Corbetta et al. 2005; Committeri et al. 2007) and, at the same time, discloses a fine anatomical and functional congruency between the white matter hodological and the gray-matter topological lesional components of spatial neglect, thus reaffirming the crucial role played by the integrated functioning of the right hemispheric parietal–frontal network in spatial orienting and awareness.

The TPJ and the middle frontal gyrus belong to a ventral attentional network that shows an increase in the BOLD when attention is dragged to unexpected visual events (Downar et al. 2000; Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Serences et al. 2005; Asplund et al. 2010). Based on the finding of a selective BOLD activation of the right TPJ and the middle frontal gyrus in response to targets presented at attentionally unattended-invalid spatial locations, Corbetta and Shulman (2002) have proposed that both the attentional disturbances and the higher frequency of spatial neglect after right hemispheric stroke depend on the anatomical and functional disruption of this ventral attentional network. Because of anatomical proximity and reciprocal connectivity, damage of the right ventral network would produce hypoactivation of an adjacent dorsal attentional system composed by the intraparietal sulcus and the frontal eye field areas. Due to reciprocal callosal inhibition, this hypoactivation would release hyperactivation in homologous dorsal areas in the left hemisphere with a pathologically increased rightward attentional bias. This theoretical approach establishes coherence between fMRI and anatomo-clinical findings and opens up new interesting points of discussion and investigation. As an example, one might still argue that since in this model only the role of the right temporo-parietal junction and the middle frontal gyrus is emphasized for attentional disengagement and reorienting, then lesion in the right ventral network should produce a generalized slowing of attentional disengagement. This seems not entirely congruent with the behavior of spatial neglect patients that are well characterized by prevalent defective disengagement from ipsi- to contralesional space rather than from contra- to ipsilesional space. However, residual disengagement abilities in spatial neglect patients could be explained by fMRI findings, showing that also the left TPJ–IFG has an important BOLD response to invalid targets and stimulus driven orienting (Doricchi et al. 2010). This might suggest that in spatial neglect patients slowed reorienting toward invalid targets presented in the left visual field might be due to preferred response of the left ventral network to invalid targets in the right visual space, or to the fact that right brain damage precludes or slows down the processing of invalid targets presented in the left visual space to the spared reorienting network in the left hemisphere.

It is finally important to stress that different cytoarchitectonic areas form the region that is usually labeled temporal parietal junction: Brodmann area (BA) 40 (corresponding to areas PF, PFt, PFop, PFcm in Economo and Koskinas' atlas; Economo and Koskinas 1925; see also Tomaiuolo and Petrides 2010), BA 39 (corresponding to PG), the caudal of BA 22 (corresponding to TA1), and BA 37 (corresponding to PH, PHP, PHT, PHO). Future studies should clarify whether the different cytoarchitectonic areas forming the temporal parietal junction have different roles in attentional orienting and whether, in case of brain damage, they equally contribute or not to the occurrence of the spatial neglect syndrome.

Subcortical Thalamic Spatial Neglect

A small percentage of spatial neglect patients showed a small lesion sparing the fronto-parietal segment of the arcuate fasciculus and its projections. These patients showed similar clinical signs as the more typical spatial neglect patients with fronto-parietal disconnection. Our analysis revealed that lesions in these patients were centered on the thalamus and its adjacent white matter. Spatial neglect has previously been reported in humans with a lesion in the thalamus (Watson and Heilman 1979; Cambier et al. 1980; Schott et al. 1981; Rafal and Posner 1987; Mesulam 1999) and has been associated with a target engagement deficit in the contralesional hemifield (Rafal and Posner 1987). Nearly all incoming information to the cortex is directed through the thalamus (Behrens et al. 2003). However, in this small percentage, the lesion was found on the latero-dorsal portion of the thalamus, including the posterior and lateral dorsal thalamic nuclei and the anterior part of the pulvinar (Morel et al. 1997; Nieuwenhuys et al. 2008). The lateral posterior nucleus and the pulvinar receive their mains afferent fibers from the superior colliculus and pretectum, known as the visuomotor complex of the midbrain (Trojanowski and Jacobson 1975; Bender 1981; Benevento and Standage 1983; Huerta and Harting 1983; Lysakowski et al. 1986). Apart from the above, the lateral posterior nucleus and the pulvinar also receive input from the motor and premotor cortex (Romansky et al. 1997). Lateral posterior nucleus and pulvinar send efferent fibers toward the superior and inferior parietal lobules (Burton and Jones 1976), while the lateral dorsal nucleus is reciprocally connected with the entire prefrontal cortex including, in particular, the frontal eye field (Tanaka 1976; Huerta and Harting 1983; Schell and Strick 1984; Goldman-Rakic and Porrino 1985; Russchen et al. 1987). The posterior and lateral dorsal thalamic nuclei and the anterior part of the pulvinar can be considered as association nuclei characterized by strong reciprocal connections with the association areas (Nieuwenhuys et al. 2008). It is suggested that these nuclei relay high-order cortico-cortical communication and modulate functions requiring visual–sensory–motor integration (Sherman and Guillery 2002). A disconnection of these nuclei might thus induce an indirect fronto-parietal dysfunction with consequent spatial neglect.

Caveats and Conclusions

Some notes of caution must be considered for the present study. First, the white matter atlas that we have used is based on anatomical information gathered from a population of normal subjects aged from 18 to 22 years, while the stroke population we studied was much older. A decrease in the size of the pathways reconstructed with tractography in relation to age has been previously reported (Stadlbauer et al. 2008). White matter pathways also show a descending gradient of intersubjects variability going from the stem portion (>90% of the population studied) of the white matter pathways to the most peripheral zones (<50% of the population studied; Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011). In our analysis, we chose a probability >50% in order to consider only the almost invariable anatomical core of each single tract and not its periphery (Thiebaut de Schotten, ffytche, et al. 2011).

A second note of caution concerns the possible contribution of interhemispheric callosal disconnection to MCA spatial neglect. At present, DTI tractography poorly reconstructs crossing fibers, with a consequent underestimation of the lateral projections of the cortico-spinal tract and the corpus callosum in the atlas we used. Therefore, the present study does not allow excluding that disconnection of callosal fibers also contributes to spatial neglect following vascular damage in the MCA (Bartolomeo et al. 2007).

In conclusion, this study confirms that lesion of white matter pathways is a relevant determinant of chronic MCA spatial neglect. When the integrated interplay between the right frontal and parietal lobes is interrupted by disconnection (direct anatomical–functional damage) or by thalamic infarction (indirect functional damage), chronic spatial neglect is likely to occur. In addition, we were able to provide advances in the understanding of the role of disconnection in spatial neglect, by pinpointing the role of distinct SLF branches. At variance with the proposals made by Karnath et al. (2001, 2004, 2009, 2011), stressing the importance of purely cortical damage, we conclude that chronicity of left spatial neglect in the post-acute phase of MCA stroke is linked to the anatomical and functional disruption of parietal–frontal white matter connections and not linked to damage of the central sector of the superior temporal gyrus.

Funding

F.D. was supported by a grant from the Fondazione Santa Lucia IRCCS. M.T.d.S. was supported by the Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowships for Career Development (FP7) and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR; project CAFORPFC, number ANR-09-RPDOC-004-01 and project HM-TC, number ANR-09-EMER-006).

Notes

We thank Dr Mara Cercignani and Dr Marco Bozzali for help in the acquisition of DTI data and Flavio Dell'Acqua for the help with the Spherical Deconvolution analysis. Conflict of Interest: None declared.

References

Aiello
M
Jacquin-Courtois
S
Merola
S
Ottaviani
T
Tomaiuolo
F
Bueti
D
Rossetti
Y
Doricchi
F
No inherent left and right side in human ‘mental number line’: evidence from right brain damage
Brain
2012
Alexander
DC
An introduction to computational diffusion MRI: the diffusion tensor and beyond
Visualization and processing of tensor fields.
2006
Berlin
Springer
(pg. 
83
-
106
)
Asplund
CL
Todd
JJ
Snyder
AP
Marois
R
A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention
Nat Neurosci
2010
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
507
-
514
)
Azouvi
P
Samuel
C
Louis-Dreyfus
A
Bernati
T
Bartolomeo
P
Beis
JM
Chokron
S
Leclercq
M
Marchal
F
Martin
Y
, et al. 
(GEREN/GRECO) FCSGoAoUN
Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr
2002
, vol. 
73
 (pg. 
160
-
166
)
Bartolomeo
P
Visual neglect
Curr Opin Neurol
2007
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
381
-
386
)
Bartolomeo
P
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Doricchi
F
Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome
Cereb Cortex
2007
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
2479
-
2490
)
Basser
PJ
Pierpaoli
C
Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI
J Magn Reson B
1996
, vol. 
111
 (pg. 
209
-
219
)
Bates
E
Wilson
S
Saygin
A
Dick
F
Sereno
MI
Knight
RT
Dronkers
NF
Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping
Nat Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
448
-
450
)
Behrens
TE
Johansen-Berg
H
Woolrich
MW
Smith
SM
Wheeler-Kingshott
CA
Boulby
PA
Barker
GJ
Sillery
EL
Sheehan
K
Ciccarelli
O
, et al. 
Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging
Nat Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
750
-
757
)
Bender
DB
Retinotopic organization of macaque pulvinar
J Neurophysiol
1981
, vol. 
46
 (pg. 
672
-
693
)
Benevento
LA
Standage
GP
The organization of projections of the retinorecipient and nonretinorecipient nuclei of the pretectal complex and layers of the superior colliculus to the lateral pulvinar and medial pulvinar in the macaque monkey
J Comp Neurol
1983
, vol. 
217
 (pg. 
307
-
336
)
Binder
J
Marshall
R
Lazar
R
Benjamin
J
Mohr
JP
Distinct syndromes of hemineglect
Arch Neurol
1992
, vol. 
49
 (pg. 
1187
-
1194
)
Bird
C
Malhotra
P
Parton
A
Coulthard
EJ
Rushworth
MFS
Husain
M
Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr
2006
, vol. 
77
 (pg. 
1008
-
1012
)
Bogousslavsky
J
Regli
F
Assal
G
The syndrome of unilateral tuberothalamic artery territory infarction
Stroke
1986
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
434
-
441
)
Brain
RW
Visual disorientation with special reference to lesion of the right brain hemisphere
Brain
1941
, vol. 
64
 (pg. 
244
-
272
)
Bürgel
U
Amunts
K
Hoemke
L
Mohlberg
H
Gilsbach
JM
Zilles
K
White matter fiber tracts of the human brain: three-dimensional mapping at microscopic resolution, topography and intersubject variability
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
1092
-
1105
)
Burton
H
Jones
EG
The posterior thalamic region and its cortical projection in New World and Old World monkeys
J Comp Neurol
1976
, vol. 
168
 (pg. 
249
-
302
)
Cambier
J
Elghozi
D
Strube
E
Lésion du thalamus droit avec syndrome de l'hémisphère mineur. Discussion du concept de négligence thalamique
. Rev Neurol
1980
, vol. 
136
 (pg. 
105
-
116
)
Caplan
LR
Schmahmann
JD
Kase
CS
Feldmann
E
Baquis
G
Greenberg
JP
Gorelick
PB
Helgason
C
Hier
DB
Caudate infarcts
Arch Neurol
1990
, vol. 
47
 (pg. 
133
-
143
)
Catani
M
From hodology to function
Brain
2007
, vol. 
130
 (pg. 
602
-
605
)
Catani
M
Allin
MP
Husain
M
Pugliese
L
Mesulam
MM
Murray
RM
Jones
DK
Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2007
, vol. 
104
 (pg. 
17163
-
17168
)
Catani
M
Dell'acqua
F
Vergani
F
Malik
F
Hodge
H
Roy
P
Valabregue
R
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Atlas of the short frontal lobe connections: proposal for a new nomenclature
Cortex
2012
, vol. 
48
 (pg. 
273
-
291
)
Catani
M
ffytche
DH
The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes
Brain
2005
, vol. 
128
 (pg. 
2224
-
2239
)
Catani
M
Jones
DK
ffytche
DH
Perisylvian language networks of the human brain
Ann Neurol
2005
, vol. 
57
 (pg. 
8
-
16
)
Catani
M
Mesulam
M
The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: history and current state
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
953
-
961
)
Catani
M
Mesulam
MM
What is a disconnection syndrome?
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
911
-
913
)
Catani
M
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Atlas of human brain connections
2012
Oxford
Oxford University Press
Catani
M
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
1105
-
1132
)
Chung
CS
Caplan
LR
Han
W
Pessin
MS
Lee
KH
Kim
JM
Thalamic haemorrhage
Brain
1996
, vol. 
119
 
Pt 6
(pg. 
1873
-
1886
)
Ciaraffa
F
Castelli
G
Parati
EA
Bartolomeo
P
Bizzi
A
Visual neglect as a disconnection syndrome? A confirmatory case report
Neurocase
2012
 
Epub ahead of print http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2012.667130
Committeri
G
Pitzalis
S
Galati
G
Patria
F
Pelle
G
Sabatini
U
Castriota-Scanderbeg
A
Piccardi
L
Guariglia
C
Pizzamiglio
L
Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans
Brain
2007
, vol. 
130
 (pg. 
431
-
441
)
Corbetta
M
Kincade
M
Lewis
C
Snyder
A
Sapir
A
Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect
Nat Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
1603
-
1610
)
Corbetta
M
Shulman
GL
Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain
Nat Rev Neurosci
2002
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
201
-
215
)
Critchley
M
The Parietal lobes
1953
London
Edward Arnold
Damasio
AR
Damasio
H
Chui
HC
Neglect following damage to frontal lobe or basal ganglia
Neuropsychologia
1980
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
123
-
132
)
Dejerine
J
Contribution a l’étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des differentes variétés de cécité-verbale
Mem Soc Biol
1892
, vol. 
4
 (pg. 
61
-
90
)
Dejerine
J
Sur un cas de cécité verbale avec agraphie suivie d'autopsie
Mem Soc Biol
1891
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
197
-
201
)
Dell'Acqua
F
Coward
J
Simmons
A
Murphy
DGM
Williams
S
Catani
M
Mapping crossing fibres of the human brain with spherical deconvolution: towards an atlas for clinico-anatomical correlation studies
Proc Int Soc Magn Reson Med
2009
, vol. 
17
 pg. 
3562
 
Dell'acqua
F
Scifo
P
Rizzo
G
Catani
M
Simmons
A
Scotti
G
Fazio
F
A modified damped Richardson-Lucy algorithm to reduce isotropic background effects in spherical deconvolution
Neuroimage
2010
, vol. 
49
 (pg. 
1446
-
1458
)
Dell'acqua
F
Simmons
A
Williams
S
Catani
M
Can spherical deconvolution provide more information than fiber orientations? Hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, a true-tract specific index to characterize white matter diffusion
Hum Brain Mapp
2012
De Witte
L
Verhoeven
J
Engelborghs
S
De Deyn
PP
Mariën
P
Crossed aphasia and visuo-spatial neglect following a right thalamic stroke: a case study and review of the literature
Behav Neurol
2008
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
177
-
194
)
Diller
L
Ben-Yishay
Y
Gerstman
L
Goodkin
R
Gordon
W
Weinberg
J
Studies in cognition and rehabilitation in hemiplegia
1974
New York
University Medical Center
Donnan
GA
Bladin
PF
Berkovic
SF
Longley
WA
Saling
MM
The stroke syndrome of striatocapsular infarction
Brain
1991
, vol. 
114
 
Pt 1A
(pg. 
51
-
70
)
Doricchi
F
Angelelli
P
Misrepresentation of horizontal space in left unilateral neglect: role of hemianopia
Neurology
1999
, vol. 
52
 
9
(pg. 
1845
-
1852
)
Doricchi
F
Guariglia
P
Figliozzi
F
Silvetti
M
Bruno
G
Gasparini
M
Causes of cross-over in unilateral neglect: between-group comparisons, within-patient dissociations and eye movements
Brain
2005
, vol. 
128
 
Pt 6
(pg. 
1386
-
1406
)
Doricchi
F
Macci
E
Silvetti
M
Macaluso
E
Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the Posner task
Cereb Cortex
2010
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
1574
-
1585
)
Doricchi
F
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Tomaiuolo
F
Bartolomeo
P
White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
983
-
995
)
Doricchi
F
Tomaiuolo
F
The anatomy of neglect without hemianopia: a key role for parietal-frontal disconnection
Neuroreport
2003
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
2239
-
2243
)
Downar
J
Crawley
AP
Mikulis
DJ
Davis
KD
A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment
Nat Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
277
-
283
)
Economo
C
Koskinas
GN
Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde des erwachsenen Menschen
1925
Wien, Berlin
Springer
Ferro
JM
Kertesz
A
Black
SE
Subcortical neglect: quantitation, anatomy, and recovery
Neurology
1987
, vol. 
37
 (pg. 
1487
-
1492
)
Finger
S
Koehler
PJ
Jagella
C
The Monakow concept of diaschisis: origins and perspectives
Arch Neurol
2004
, vol. 
61
 (pg. 
283
-
288
)
Gaffan
D
Hornak
J
Visual neglect in the monkey. Representation and disconnection
. Brain
1997
, vol. 
120
 
Pt 9
(pg. 
1647
-
1657
)
Gainotti
G
Messerli
P
Tissot
R
Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1972
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
545
-
550
)
Genovese
CR
Lazar
NA
Nichols
T
Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
870
-
878
)
Geschwind
N
Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man
Brain
1965
, vol. 
88
 (pg. 
585
-
644
)
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Porrino
LJ
The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe
J Comp Neurol
1985
, vol. 
242
 (pg. 
535
-
560
)
Graff-Radford
NR
Damasio
H
Yamada
T
Eslinger
PJ
Damasio
AR
Nonhaemorrhagic thalamic infarction. Clinical, neuropsychological and electrophysiological findings in four anatomical groups defined by computerized tomography
Brain
1985
, vol. 
108
 
Pt 2
(pg. 
485
-
516
)
Halligan
PW
Marshall
JC
Wade
DT
Do visual field deficits exacerbate visuo-spatial neglect?
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1990
, vol. 
53
 (pg. 
487
-
491
)
He
BJ
Snyder
A
Vincent
JL
Epstein
A
Shulman
GL
Corbetta
M
Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect
Neuron
2007
, vol. 
53
 (pg. 
905
-
918
)
Healton
EB
Navarro
C
Bressman
S
Brust
JC
Subcortical neglect
Neurology
1982
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
776
-
778
)
Heilman
KM
Valenstein
E
Frontal lobe neglect in man
Neurology
1972
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
660
-
664
)
Hier
DB
Davis
KR
Richardson
EP
Mohr
JP
Hypertensive putaminal hemorrhage
Ann Neurol
1977
, vol. 
1
 (pg. 
152
-
159
)
Hillis
AE
Barker
PB
Beauchamp
NJ
Gordon
B
Wityk
RJ
MR perfusion imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and neglect
Neurology
2000
, vol. 
55
 (pg. 
782
-
788
)
Hirose
G
Kosoegawa
H
Saeki
M
Kitagawa
Y
Oda
R
Kanda
S
Matsuhira
T
The syndrome of posterior thalamic hemorrhage
Neurology
1985
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
998
-
1002
)
Holmes
CJ
Hoge
R
Collins
L
Woods
R
Toga
AW
Evans
AC
Enhancement of MR images using registration for signal averaging
J Comput Assist Tomogr
1998
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
324
-
333
)
Huerta
MF
Harting
JK
Sublamination within the superficial gray layer of the squirrel monkey: an analysis of the tectopulvinar projection using anterograde and retrograde transport methods
Brain Res
1983
, vol. 
261
 (pg. 
119
-
126
)
Husain
M
Kennard
C
Distractor-dependent frontal neglect
Neuropsychologia
1997
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
829
-
841
)
Husain
M
Kennard
C
Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction
J Neurol
1996
, vol. 
243
 (pg. 
652
-
657
)
Husain
M
Mattingley
JB
Rorden
C
Kennard
C
Driver
J
Distinguishing sensory and motor biases in parietal and frontal neglect
Brain
2000
, vol. 
123
 
Pt 8
(pg. 
1643
-
1659
)
Husain
M
Nachev
P
Space and the parietal cortex
Trends Cogn Sci
2007
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
30
-
36
)
Jones
DK
Determining and visualizing uncertainty in estimates of fiber orientation from diffusion tensor MRI
Magn Reson Med
2003
, vol. 
49
 (pg. 
7
-
12
)
Jones
DK
The effect of gradient sampling schemes on measures derived from diffusion tensor MRI: a Monte Carlo study
Magn Reson Med
2004
, vol. 
51
 (pg. 
807
-
815
)
Jones
DK
Studying connections in the living human brain with diffusion MRI
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
936
-
952
)
Jones
DK
Griffin
LD
Alexander
DC
Catani
M
Horsfield
MA
Howard
R
Williams
SC
Spatial normalization and averaging of diffusion tensor MRI data sets
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
592
-
617
)
Karnath
H-O
Ferber
S
Himmelbach
M
Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe
Nature
2001
, vol. 
411
 (pg. 
950
-
963
)
Karnath
H-O
Himmelbach
M
Rorden
C
The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar
Brain
2002
, vol. 
125
 (pg. 
350
-
360
)
Karnath
H-O
Rennig
J
Johannsen
L
Rorden
C
The anatomy underlying acute versus chronic spatial neglect: a longitudinal study
Brain
2011
, vol. 
134
 (pg. 
903
-
912
)
Karnath
HO
Fruhmann Berger
M
Küker
W
Rorden
C
The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients
Cereb Cortex
2004
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
1164
-
1172
)
Karnath
HO
Rorden
C
Ticini
LF
Damage to white matter fiber tracts in acute spatial neglect
Cereb Cortex
2009
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
2331
-
2337
)
Karussis
D
Leker
RR
Abramsky
O
Cognitive dysfunction following thalamic stroke: a study of 16 cases and review of the literature
J Neurol Sci
2000
, vol. 
172
 (pg. 
25
-
29
)
Kempler
D
Metter
EJ
Jackson
CA
Hanson
WR
Riege
WH
Mazziotta
JC
Phelps
ME
Disconnection and cerebral metabolism. The case of conduction aphasia
Arch Neurol
1988
, vol. 
45
 (pg. 
275
-
279
)
Kumral
E
Kocaer
T
Ertübey
NO
Kumral
K
Thalamic hemorrhage. A prospective study of 100 patients
Stroke
1995
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
964
-
970
)
Khurshid
S
Trupe
LA
Newhart
M
Davis
C
Molitoris
JJ
Medina
J
Leigh
R
Hillis
AE
Reperfusion of specific cortical areas is associated with improvement in distinct forms of hemispatial neglect
Cortex
2012
, vol. 
48
 (pg. 
530
-
539
)
Lawes
INC
Barrick
TR
Murugam
V
Spierings
N
Evans
DR
Song
M
Clark
CA
Atlas-based segmentation of white matter tracts of the human brain using diffusion tensor tractography and comparison with classical dissection
Neuroimage
2008
, vol. 
39
 (pg. 
62
-
79
)
Leemans
A
Jones
DK
The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data
Magn Reson Med
2009
, vol. 
61
 (pg. 
1336
-
1349
)
Leibovitch
FS
Black
SE
Caldwell
CB
Ebert
PL
Ehrlich
LE
Szalai
JP
Brain-behavior correlations in hemispatial neglect using CT and SPECT: the Sunnybrook Stroke Study
Neurology
1998
, vol. 
50
 (pg. 
901
-
908
)
Levine
DN
Warach
JD
Benowitz
LI
Calvanio
R
Left spatial neglect: effects of lesion size and premorbid brain atrophy on severity and recovery following right cerebral infarction
Neurology
1986
, vol. 
36
 (pg. 
362
-
366
)
Lysakowski
A
Standage
GP
Benevento
LA
Histochemical and architectonic differentiation of zones of pretectal and collicular inputs to the pulvinar and dorsal lateral geniculate nuclei in the macaque
J Comp Neurol
1986
, vol. 
250
 (pg. 
431
-
448
)
Marquardt
D
An algorithm for least-squares estimation of nonlinear parameters
Siam J Appl Math
1963
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
431
-
441
)
Mcfie
J
Piercy
MF
Zangwill
OL
Visual-spatial agnosia associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere
Brain
1950
, vol. 
73
 (pg. 
167
-
190
)
Mesulam
MM
A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect
Ann Neurol
1981
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
309
-
325
)
Mesulam
MM
Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
1999
, vol. 
354
 (pg. 
1325
-
1346
)
Mesulam
MM
Geschwind
N
On the possible role of neocortex and its limbic connections in the process of attention and schizophrenia: clinical cases of inattention in man and experimental anatomy in monkey
J Psychiatr Res
1978
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
249
-
259
)
Monakow
CV
Die Lokalisation im Grosshirn und der Funktion durch kortikale Herde
1914
Wiesbaden
Verlag von J.F. Bergmann
Morel
A
Magnin
M
Jeanmonod
D
Multiarchitectonic and stereotactic atlas of the human thalamus
J Comp Neurol
1997
, vol. 
387
 (pg. 
588
-
630
)
Mori
S
Oishi
K
Jiang
H
Li
X
Akhter
K
Hua
K
Faria
AV
Mahmood
A
Woods
R
Toga
AW
, et al. 
Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template
Neuroimage
2008
, vol. 
40
 (pg. 
570
-
582
)
Mort
DJ
Malhotra
P
Mannan
SK
Rorden
C
Pambakian
A
Kennard
C
Husain
M
The anatomy of visual neglect
Brain
2003
, vol. 
126
 (pg. 
1986
-
1997
)
Motomura
N
Yamadori
A
Mori
E
Ogura
J
Sakai
T
Sawada
T
Unilateral spatial neglect due to hemorrhage in the thalamic region
Acta Neurol Scand
1986
, vol. 
74
 (pg. 
190
-
194
)
Nieuwenhuys
R
Voogd
J
van Huijzen
C
The human central nervous system
2008
Berlin, NY
Springer
Oishi
K
Zilles
K
Amunts
K
Faria
A
Jiang
H
Li
X
Akhter
K
Hua
K
Woods
R
Toga
AW
, et al. 
Human brain white matter atlas: identification and assignment of common anatomical structures in superficial white matter
Neuroimage
2008
, vol. 
43
 (pg. 
447
-
57
)
Oxbury
JM
Campbell
DC
Oxbury
SM
Unilateral spatial neglect and impairments of spatial analysis and visual perception
Brain
1974
, vol. 
97
 (pg. 
551
-
564
)
Park
KC
Lee
BH
Kim
EJ
Shin
MH
Choi
KM
Yoon
SS
Kwon
SU
Chung
CS
Lee
KH
Heilman
KM
, et al. 
Deafferentation-disconnection neglect induced by posterior cerebral artery infarction
Neurology
2006
, vol. 
66
 (pg. 
56
-
61
)
Paterson
A
Zangwill
O
Disorders of visual space perception associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere
Brain
1944
, vol. 
67
 (pg. 
331
-
358
)
Perani
D
Vallar
G
Cappa
S
Messa
C
Fazio
F
Aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke. A clinical/cerebral perfusion correlation study
Brain
1987
, vol. 
110
 (pg. 
1211
-
1229
)
Pizzamiglio
L
Judica
A
Razzano
C
Zoccolotti
P
Toward a comprehensive diagnosis of visuo-spatial disorders in unilateral brain-damaged patients
Psychol Assess
1989
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
199
-
218
)
Price
CJ
Warburton
EA
Moore
CJ
Frackowiak
RSJ
Friston
KJ
Dynamic diaschisis: anatomically remote and context-sensitive human brain lesions
J Cogn Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
419
-
429
)
Rafal
RD
Posner
MI
Deficits in human visual spatial attention following thalamic lesions
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1987
, vol. 
84
 (pg. 
7349
-
7353
)
Romansky
LM
Giguere
M
Bates
JF
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Topographic organization of medial pulvinar connections with the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey
J Comp Neurol
1997
, vol. 
379
 (pg. 
313
-
332
)
Rorden
C
Bonilha
L
Nichols
TE
Rank-order versus mean based statistics for neuroimaging
Neuroimage
2007
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
1531
-
1537
)
Rorden
C
Karnath
HO
Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age?
Nat Rev Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
813
-
819
)
Rudrauf
D
Mehta
S
Grabowski
TJ
Disconnection's renaissance takes shape: formal incorporation in group-level lesion studies
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
1084
-
1096
)
Russchen
FT
Amaral
DG
Price
JL
The afferent input to the magnocellular division of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the monkey, Macaca fascicularis
J Comp Neurol
1987
, vol. 
256
 (pg. 
175
-
210
)
Schell
GR
Strick
PL
The origin of thalamic inputs to the arcuate premotor and supplementary motor areas
J Neurosci
1984
, vol. 
4
 (pg. 
539
-
560
)
Schmahmann
JD
Pandya
DN
Fiber pathways of the brain
2006
Oxford
Oxford University Press
Schott
B
Laurent
B
Mauguiere
F
Chazot
G
Motor negligence in a case of right thalamic hematoma
Rev Neurol
1981
, vol. 
137
 (pg. 
447
-
455
)
Serences
JT
Shomstein
S
Leber
AB
Golay
X
Egeth
HE
Yantis
S
Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex
Psychol Sci
2005
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
114
-
122
)
Sherman
SM
Guillery
RW
The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
2002
, vol. 
357
 (pg. 
1695
-
1708
)
Shinoura
N
Suzuki
Y
Yamada
R
Tabei
Y
Saito
K
Yagi
K
Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect
Neuropsychologia
2009
, vol. 
47
 (pg. 
2600
-
2603
)
Shomstein
S
Lee
J
Behrmann
M
Top-down and bottom-up attentional guidance: investigating the role of the dorsal and ventral parietal cortices
Exp Brain Res
2010
, vol. 
206
 (pg. 
197
-
208
)
Stadlbauer
A
Salomonowitz
E
Strunk
G
Hammen
T
Ganslandt
O
Age-related degradation in the central nervous system: assessment with diffusion-tensor imaging and quantitative fiber tracking
Radiology
2008
, vol. 
247
 (pg. 
179
-
188
)
Stein
S
Volpe
BT
Classical “parietal” neglect syndrome after subcortical right frontal lobe infarction
Neurology
1983
, vol. 
33
 (pg. 
797
-
799
)
Stone
SP
Patel
P
Greenwood
RJ
Halligan
PW
Measuring visual neglect in acute stroke and predicting its recovery: the visual neglect recovery index
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr
1992
, vol. 
55
 (pg. 
431
-
436
)
Tanaka
D
Thalamic projections of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)
Brain Res
1976
, vol. 
110
 (pg. 
21
-
38
)
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Dell'Acqua
F
Forkel
SJ
Simmons
A
Vergani
F
Murphy
DGM
Catani
M
A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention
Nat Neurosci
2011
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
1245
-
1246
)
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Dell'acqua
F
Valabregue
R
Catani
M
Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts
Cortex
2012
, vol. 
48
 (pg. 
82
-
96
)
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
ffytche
DH
Bizzi
A
Dell'Acqua
F
Allin
M
Walshe
M
Murray
R
Williams
SC
Murphy
DG
Catani
M
Atlasing location, asymmetry and inter-subject variability of white matter tracts in the human brain with MR diffusion tractography
Neuroimage
2011
, vol. 
54
 (pg. 
49
-
59
)
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Kinkingnehun
S
Delmaire
C
Lehericy
S
Duffau
H
Thivard
L
Volle
E
Levy
R
Dubois
B
Bartolomeo
P
Visualization of disconnection syndromes in humans
Cortex
2008
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
1097
-
1103
)
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Urbanski
M
Duffau
H
Volle
E
Levy
R
Dubois
B
Bartolomeo
P
Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans
Science
2005
, vol. 
309
 (pg. 
2226
-
2228
)
Tomaiuolo
F
Petrides
M
Citoarchitettonica cerebrale umana: Il contributo di un vecchio atlante di anatomia all'acquisizione delle nuove conoscenze sul cervello
2010
Italia
Springer-Verlag
Tomaiuolo
F
Voci
L
Bresci
M
Cozza
S
Posteraro
F
Oliva
M
Doricchi
F
Selective visual neglect in right brain damaged patients with splenial interhemispheric disconnection
Exp Brain Res
2010
, vol. 
206
 (pg. 
209
-
217
)
Trojanowski
JQ
Jacobson
S
A combined horseradish peroxidase-autoradiographic investigation of reciprocal connections between superior temporal gyrus and pulvinar in squirrel monkey
Brain Res
1975
, vol. 
85
 (pg. 
347
-
353
)
Tsang
JM
Dougherty
RF
Deutsch
GK
Wandell
BA
Ben-Shachar
M
Frontoparietal white matter diffusion properties predict mental arithmetic skills in children
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2009
, vol. 
106
 (pg. 
22546
-
22551
)
Urbanski
M
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Rodrigo
S
Catani
M
Oppenheim
C
Touzé
E
Chokron
S
Méder
JF
Levy
R
Dubois
B
, et al. 
Brain networks of spatial awareness: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging tractography
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr
2008
, vol. 
79
 (pg. 
598
-
601
)
Urbanski
M
Thiebaut de Schotten
M
Rodrigo
S
Oppenheim
C
Touzé
E
Méder
J-F
Moreau
K
Loeper-Jeny
C
Dubois
B
Bartolomeo
P
DTI-MR tractography of white matter damage in stroke patients with neglect
Exp Brain Res
2011
, vol. 
208
 (pg. 
491
-
505
)
Vallar
G
Perani
D
The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man
Neuropsychologia
1986
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
609
-
622
)
Verdon
V
Schwartz
S
Lovblad
KO
Hauert
CA
Vuilleumier
P
Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping
Brain
2010
, vol. 
133
 (pg. 
880
-
894
)
Watson
RT
Heilman
KM
Thalamic neglect
Neurology
1979
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
690
-
694
)
Watson
RT
Heilman
KM
Miller
BD
King
FA
Neglect after mesencephalic reticular formation lesions
Neurology
1974
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
294
-
298
)
Watson
RT
Miller
BD
Heilman
KM
Nonsensory neglect
Ann Neurol
1978
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
505
-
508
)
Watson
RT
Valenstein
E
Heilman
KM
Thalamic neglect. Possible role of the medial thalamus and nucleus reticularis in behavior
Arch Neurol
1981
, vol. 
38
 (pg. 
501
-
506
)
Waxman
SG
Ricaurte
GA
Tucker
SB
Thalamic hemorrhage with neglect and memory disorder
J Neurol Sci
1986
, vol. 
75
 (pg. 
105
-
112
)
Wedeen
VJ
Wang
RP
Schmahmann
JD
Benner
T
Tseng
WY
Dai
G
Pandya
DN
Hagmann
P
D'arceuil
HE
De Crespigny
AJ
Diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging (DSI) tractography of crossing fibers
Neuroimage
2008
, vol. 
41
 (pg. 
1267
-
1277
)
Weiller
C
Ringelstein
EB
Reiche
W
Thron
A
Buell
U
The large striatocapsular infarct. A clinical and pathophysiological entity
Arch Neurol
1990
, vol. 
47
 (pg. 
1085
-
1091
)
Weiller
C
Willmes
K
Reiche
W
Thron
A
Isensee
C
Buell
U
Ringelstein
EB
The case of aphasia or neglect after striatocapsular infarction
Brain
1993
, vol. 
116
 (pg. 
1509
-
1525
)
Weintraub
S
Mesulam
MM
Right cerebral dominance in spatial attention. Further evidence based on ipsilateral neglect
Arch Neurol
1987
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
621
-
625
)
Yasaka
M
Read
SJ
O'Keefe
GJ
Egan
GF
Pointon
O
McKay
WJ
Donnan
GA
Positron emission tomography in ischaemic stroke: cerebral perfusion and metabolism after stroke onset
J Clin Neurosci
1998
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
413
-
416
)