Skip to main content
Log in

Higher order affordances

  • Theoretical/Review
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Affordances are opportunities for action for a given animal (or animals) in a given environment or situation. The concept of affordance has been widely adopted in the behavioral sciences, but important questions remain. We propose a new way of understanding the nature of affordances; in particular, how affordances are related to one another. We claim that many – perhaps most – affordances emerge from non-additive relations among other affordances, such that some affordances are of higher order relative to other affordances. That is, we propose that affordances form a continuous category of perceiveables that differ only in whether and how they relate to other affordances. We argue that: (1) opportunities for behaviors of all kinds can be described as affordances, (2) some affordances emerge from relations between animal and environment, whereas most affordances emerge from relations between other affordances, and (3) all affordances lawfully structure ambient energy arrays and, therefore, can be perceived directly. Our concept of higher order affordances provides a general account of behavioral phenomena that traditionally have been interpreted in terms of cognitive processes (e.g., remembering or imagining) as well as behavioral phenomena that have traditionally been interpreted in terms of cultural rules, such as conventions, or customs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig.1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Reproduced with permission from Peker et al. (2023, p. 626)

Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Reproduced with permission from Peker et al. (2023, p. 629)

Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Not applicable.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Bruineberg et al. (2019; cf. Chemero, 2006) referred to the fact that social conventions can have different values in terms of contingency, using that term in its statistical or probabilistic sense. We regard this as improper reification: Used in this way, the term presumes the absence (or unknowability) of lawful relations (cf. Sprenger, 2018).

  2. As noted by many authors (e.g., Fajen et al., 2009; J. J. Gibson, 1979; Reed, 1996), as part of the ontology affordances exist independent of perception. That is, affordances exist whether or not they are perceived. Similarly, affordances exist independent of intention; that is, affordances for an action exist whether or not an animal (or animals) care about that action.

  3. The concept of affordance was developed to account for the existence of meaning in animal-environment systems. If, as J. J. Gibson (1979) claimed, affordances are real things that are lawfully specified and which might (therefore) be directly perceived, then we can wonder what motivation there could be, in a Darwinian system, for the perception of anything that is not an affordance (e.g., Stoffregen, 2000, 2004). However, while we regard this question as being important, it is not central to our arguments in this article.

  4. Our concept of higher order affordances resembles the argument of Reed (1996, p. 123): “Our extensive use of tools, transport and modifications of materials, and deployment of complex sequences of resource appropriation (often shared across individuals) put an emphasis on the discovery of relationships among affordances, not just on isolated affordances themselves,” emphasis in the original. From our perspective, “relationships among affordances” are higher order affordances. A similar concept has been proposed by Boschker et al. (2002, p. 34), “Our thesis is that one can perceive affordances of different grains or, in other words, that meaning is directly picked up … as units of information that may vary in scale. Affordances of coarse grain may be regarded as emerging from and constrained by affordances of finer grain, suggesting a nested structure (hierarchy) of perceived action possibilities. One should not assume that the relation between affordances of different grains is deterministic or causal. Presumably, that relation is emergent in nature; that is, higher level affordances emerge from lower ones.”

  5. We recognize that many affordances, both lower and higher order, differ qualitatively, and that the existence of qualitatively differed types of affordances is critical to (for example) many aspects of “higher cognition.” Yet we maintain that all affordances have the same ontological status, and that all affordances are lawfully specified, such that they can be directly perceived. This issue is discussed in later sections.

  6. The emergence of higher order affordances from other, lower order affordances undermines proposals that there can be conflict between distinct affordances (e.g., Kalénine et al. 2016). Affordances that appear to be in conflict may, in fact, relate to each other to form other affordances of higher order.

  7. Our claim is not mentalist (Sperry, 1980). That is, we do not claim that consciousness has any causal influence on mind-brain relations.

  8. This body of research is consistent with a more general rejection of the traditional assumption that there is a discrete boundary between “past,” “present,” and “future” (e.g., J. J. Gibson, 1979; Heft, 2020b; Stoffregen & Heath, 2022; Wagman et al., 2019a, 2019b, 2019c).

  9. Vicente and Rasmussen (1991) argued that the design of human-computer interfaces should be understood in terms of the display of information about hierarchically nested lower order and higher order affordances.

  10. Costall (2012, p. 89) pointed out that affordances for mailing a letter “could not be determined by a stranger to such things by merely peering at them in sublime isolation from other people.” This criticism is valid only if we assume that lawful specification must exist within some arbitrarily brief (and narrow) duration and location of observation. The lower order, physical affordances relating to inserting the letter into the box may be specified for an isolated, novice perceiver, but higher order affordances relating to the postal system will be specified only in patterns in ambient arrays that extend across longer durations of time, geography, interpersonal interactions, and so on.

  11. It has been suggested that the existence of information about affordances is dependent upon the intentionality of the perceiver (e.g., Rietveld et al., 2018). We support an opposing view (e.g., J. J. Gibson, 1979; Reed, 1996; Richardson et al., 2008) in which intentionality does not influence the existence of information but, instead, influences the pick-up of information. An animal’s goals (or intentions) will guide their information pick-up by structuring exploratory activity. For example, a person who wants to know how far they can reach with an unseen handheld rod typically will obtain information about this affordance by actively wielding the rod (e.g., Solomon & Turvey, 1988). Yet information about affordances can also be generated (and picked up) through activity that is outside conscious intentionality, such as standing body sway (e.g., Mark et al., 1990).

References

  • Abrams, R. A., & Balota, D. A. (1991). Mental chronometry: Beyond reaction time. Psychological Science, 2, 153–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(3, Serial No. 251).

  • Adolph, K. E. (2000). Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a veritable cliff. Psychological Science, 11, 290–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M., & Gibson, E. J. (1993). Crawling versus walking infants’ perception of affordances for locomotion over sloping surfaces. Child Development, 64, 1158–1174. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04193.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M. A., Marin, L., Weise, I., & Clearfield, M. W. (2000). Exploration in the service of prospective control. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 441–460. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00052-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., & Hoch, J. E. (2019). Motor development: Embodied, embedded, encultured, and enabling. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 141–164. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102836

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Joh, A. S., & Eppler, M. A. (2010). Infants’ perception of affordances of slopes under high- and low-friction conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2015). Motor development. In R. M. Lerner, L. S. Liben, & U. Mueller (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (pp. 113–157). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Robinson, S. R., Young, J. W., & Gill-Alvarez, F. (2008). What is the shape of developmental change? Psychological Review, 115, 527–543.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. I., Campos, J. J., Witherington, D. C., Dahl, A., Rivera, M., He, M., Uchiyama, I., & Barbu-Roth, M. (2013). The role of locomotion in psychological development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 440. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00440

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, D. W. (2007). The horse, the wheel, and language: How Bronze-age riders from the Eurasian steppes shaped the modern world. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Araújo, D., Dicks, M., & Davids, K. (2019). Selecting among affordances: A basis for channeling expertise in sport. In M. L. Cappuccio (Eds.), The MIT press handbook of embodied cognition and sport psychology. Boston: MIT Press.

  • Arthur, W. B. (2009). The Nature of Technology: What it is and how it evolves. Free Press.

  • Baber, C. (2022). Embodying design: An applied science of radical embodied cognition. MIT Press.

  • Baggs, E. (2021). All affordances are social: Foundations of a Gibsonian social ontology. Ecological Psychology, 33, 257–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2021.1965477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., & Abrams, R. A. (1995). Mental chronometry: Beyond onset latencies in the lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1289–1302.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological Psychology: Concepts and methods for studying the environment of human behavior. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwise, J., & Perry, J. (1983). Situations and Attitudes. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, S. E., & Adolph, K. E. (2003). Infants use handrails as tools in a locomotor task. Developmental Psychology, 39, 594–605. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.594

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, S. E., Adolph, K. E., & Lobo, S. A. (2005). Out of the toolbox: Toddlers differentiate wobbly and wooden handrails. Child Development, 76, 1294–1307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00851.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science, 302, 1234–1236. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1087513

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham, G. P. (2000). Events (like objects) are things, can have affordance properties, and can be perceived. Ecological Psychology, 12, 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham, G. P., Schmidt, R. C., & Rosenblum, L. D. (1989). Hefting for a maximum distance throw: A smart perceptual mechanism.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 507–528. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.15.3.507

  • Birk, R., & Manning, N. (2023). Towards an ecological social science? On introducing ‘social affordances’ to (some) social theory. Philosophical Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2023.2277346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blau, J. J. C., & Wagman, J. B. (2022). Introduction to Ecological Psychology: A lawful approach to perceiving, acting, and cognizing. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bongers, R. M., Michaels, C. F., & Smitsman, A. W. (2004). Variations of tool and task characteristics reveal that tool-use postures are anticipated. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 305–315. https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.36.3.305-315

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bongers, R. M., Smitsman, A. W., & Michaels, C. F. (2003). Geometries and dynamics of a rod determine how it is used for reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 4–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222890309602117

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boschker, M. S. J., & Bakker, F. C. (2002). Inexperienced sport climbers might perceive and utilize new opportunities for action by merely observing a model. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 95, 3–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boschker, M. S. J., Bakker, F. C., & Michaels, C. F. (2002). Memory for the functional characteristics of climbing walls: Perceiving affordances. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 25–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222890209601928

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bril, B., Rein, R., Nonaka, T., Wenban-Smith, F., & Dietrich, G. (2010). The role of expertise in tool use: Skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 825–839. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bril, B., Smaers, J., Steele, J., Rein, R., Nonaka, T., Dietrich, G., Biryukova, E., Hirata, S., & Roux, V. (2012). Functional mastery of percussive technology in nut-cracking and stone-flaking actions: experimental comparison and implications for the evolution of the human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 59–74. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Chemero, A., & Rietveld, E. (2019). General ecological information supports engagement with affordances for ‘higher’ cognition. Synthese, 196, 5231–5251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-1716-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Dolega, K., Dewhurst, J., & Baltieri, M. (2022). The Emperor’s new Markov Blanket. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45(e183), 1–76. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X21002351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Self-organization, free energy minimization, and optimal grip on a field of affordances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 599. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00599

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Rietveld, E., Parr, T., van Maanen, L., & Friston, K. J. (2018). Free-energy minimization in joint agent-environment systems: A niche construction perspective. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 455, 161–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2018.07.002

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, J., Withagen, R., & van Dijk, L. (2023). Productive pluralism: The coming of age of Ecological Psychology. Psychological Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000438

  • Bucholtz, M., & Hall, K. (2016). Embodied sociolinguistics. In N. Coupland (Ed.), Sociolinguistics: Theoretical Debates (pp. 173–197). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carello, C., Grosofsky, A., Reichel, F. D., Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1989). Visually perceiving what is reachable. Ecological Psychology, 1, 27–54. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0101_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carello, C., Vaz, D., Blau, J. J. C., & Petrusz, S. (2012). Unnerving intelligence. Ecological Psychology, 24, 241–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cesari, P., & Newell, K. M. (2000). Body-scaled transitions in human grip configurations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1657–1668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, C.-H., Wade, M. G., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2009). Perceiving affordances for aperture passage in an environment-person-person system. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 495–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2000). What events are. Ecological Psychology, 12, 37–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15, 181–195. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326969ECO1502_5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2006). Information and direct perception: A new approach. In P. Farias & J. Queiroz (Eds.), Advanced Issues on Cognitive Science and Semiotics (pp. 59-72). Shaker.

  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A., Klein, C., & Cordeiro, W. (2003). Events as changes in the layout of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15, 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326969ECO1501_02

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Complexity, hypersets, and the Ecological perspective on perception-action. Biological Theory, 2, 23–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chittka, L., & Rossi, N. (2022). Social cognition in insects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 578–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.04.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2011). Language and Other Cognitive Systems. What Is Special About Language? Language Learning and Development, 7, 263–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.584041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall, A. (1995). Socializing affordances. Theory & Psychology, 5, 467–481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354395054001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall, A. (2012). Canonical affordances in context. AVANT, 3, 85–93.

  • Crawford, L. E., Knouse, L. E., Kent, M., Vavra, D., Harding, O., LeServe, D., Fox, N., Hu, X., Li, P., Glory, C., & Lambert, K. G. (2020). Enriched environment exposure accelerates rodent driving skills. Behavioural Brain Research, 378, 112309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1881). The formation of vegetable mould through the action of worms: With observations on their habits. J. Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, T. J., Riley, M. A., Shockley, K., & Cummins-Sebree, S. (2010). Perceiving affordances for joint actions. Perception, 39, 1624–1644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diedrich, F. J., & Warren, W. H. (1995). Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 183–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doucet, C. M., Adams, I. T., & Fryxell, J. M. (1994). Beaver dam and cache composition: Are woody species used differently? Ecoscience, 1, 268–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/11956860.1994.11682251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2009). Tool use and physical cognition in birds and mammals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 27–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenburg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fajen, B. R. (2005). The Scaling of Information to Action in Visually Guided Braking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 1107–1123. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.1107

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fajen, B. R., & Matthis, J. S. (2011). Direct perception of action-scaled affordances: The shrinking gap problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1442–1457. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023510

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2009). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40, 79–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faugloire, E., Bardy, B. G., Merhi, O., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2005). Exploring coordination dynamics of the postural system with real-time visual feedback. Neuroscience Letters, 374, 136–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fellers, H., & Fellers, G. M. (1976). Tool use in a social insect and its implications for competitive interactions. Science, 192, 70–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiore, D. (2020). The art of making images: Technological affordance, design variability and labour organization in the production of engraved artefacts and body paintings in Tierra del Fuego (Southern South America). Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 27, 481–510. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-020-09474-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., & Adolph, K. E. (2014). Gut estimates: Pregnant women adapt to changing possibilities for squeezing through doorways. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 460–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., & Adolph, K. E. (2014). Affordances as probabilistic functions: Implications for development, perception, and decisions for action. Ecological Psychology, 26, 109–124.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., Celano, E. C., & Adolph, K. E. (2012). Perception of passage through openings depends on the size of the body in motion. Experimental Brain Research, 223, 301–310. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3261-y

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2018). See and be seen: Infant-caregiver social looking during locomotor free play. Developmental Science, 21, e12626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fultot, M. F., Frazier, P. A., Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (2019). What Are Nervous Systems For? Ecological Psychology, 31, 218–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gadsby, S., & Williams, D. (2018). Action, affordances, and anorexia: Body representation and basic cognition. Synthese, 195, 5297–5317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-1843-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galloway, J. C., Ryu, J.-C., & Agrawal, S. K. (2008). Babies driving robots: self-generated mobility in very young infants. Intelligent Service Robotics, 1, 123–134. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11370-007-0011-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, D. L., Mark, L. S., Ward, J. A., & Edkins, H. (2001). How do task characteristics affect the transitions between seated and standing reaches? Ecological Psychology, 13, 245–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1994). Has Psychology a future? Psychological Science, 5, 69–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1997). An ecological psychologist’s prolegomena for perceptual development: A functional approach. In C. Dent-Read & P. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development (pp. 23–45). American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (2003). The world is so full of a number of things: On specification and perceptual learning. Ecological Psychology, 15, 283–287. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1504_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Riccio, G. E., Schmuckler, M. A., Stoffregen, T. A., Rosenberg, D., & Taormina, J. (1987). Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 533–544.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review, 62(1), 32–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048826

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Givon, S., Samina, M., Ben-Shahar, O., & Segev, R. (2022). From fish out of water to new insights on navigation mechanisms. Behavioural Brain Research, 419, 113711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113711

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomer, J. A., Dash, C. H., Moore, K. S., & Pagano, C. C. (2009). Using radial outflow to provide depth information during teleoperation. Presence, 18, 304–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Good, J. M. M. (2007). The affordances for social psychology of the ecological approach to social knowing. Theory & Psychology, 17, 265–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grene, M. (1983). Empiricism and the philosophy of science, or, dogmas of empiricism. In R. S. Cohen & M. W. Wartofsky (Eds.), Epistemology, Methodology, and the Social Sciences (pp. 89–106). Reidel Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Grene, M. (1993). The primacy of the ecological self. In Ulric Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 112–117). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1994). Gibson’s affordances. Psychological Review, 101, 336–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grechkin, T. Y., Chihak, B. J., Cremer, J. F., Kearney, J. K., & Plumert, J. M. (2013). Perceiving and acting on complex affordances: how children and adults bicycle across two lanes of opposing traffic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 23–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029716

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hackney, A., & Cinelli, M. (2011). Are older adults’ actions affected by their perceptions when walking through apertures? Journal of Vision, 11, 911. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.11.911

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, B.-P., Wang, T., Lin, Q.-Q., & Dumont, H. J. (2008). Carnivory and active hunting by the planktonic testate amoeba Difflugia tuberspinifera. Hydrobiologia, 596, 197–201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-007-9096-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1989). Affordances and the body: An intentional analysis of Gibson’s “Ecological approach to visual perception.” Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 19, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1996). The ecological approach to navigation: A Gibsonian perspective. In J. Portugali (Ed.), The Construction of Cognitive Maps (pp. 105–132). Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2003). Affordances, dynamic experience, and the challenge of reification. Ecological Psychology, 15, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2007). The social constitution of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecological Psychology, 19, 85–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2012). Way-finding, navigation, and spatial cognition from a naturalist’s standpoint. In D. Waller & L. Nadel (Eds.), The Handbook of Spatial Cognition (pp. 265–294). American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2013). Environment, cognition, and culture: Reconsidering the cognitive map. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 33, 14–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2017). Perceptual information “of an entirely different order”: The “cultural environment” in the Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Ecological Psychology, 29, 122–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2018). Places: Widening the scope of an ecological approach to perception-action with an emphasis on child development. Ecological Psychology, 30, 99–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2020). Ecological psychology as social psychology? Theory & Psychology, 30, 813–826. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354320934545

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2020). Revisiting “the discovery of the occluding edge and its implications for perception” 40 years on. In J. B. Wagman & J. J. C. Blau (Eds.), Perception as information detection: Reflections on Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception (pp. 188–204). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2022). Visual art history and the psychology of perception: Perspectivism and its 20th century abandonment in the visual arts and in Gibson’s ecological psychology. Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, 57, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1002/jhbs.22115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H., Hoch, J., Edmunds, T., & Weeks, J. (2018). Can the identity of a behavior setting be perceived through patterns of joint action? An investigation of place perception. Behavioral Sciences, 4, 371–393. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs4040371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heras-Escribano, M. (2019). The Philosophy of Affordances. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-98830-6

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi, T., Cinelli, M. E., Greig, M. A., & Patla, A. E. (2006). Locomotion through apertures when wider space for locomotion is necessary: Adaptation to artificially altered bodily states. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 50–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi, T., Murai, G., Kijima, A., Seya, Y., Wagman, J. B., & Imanaka, K. (2011). Athletic experience influences shoulder rotations when running through apertures. Human Movement Science, 30, 534–549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. A., Bentley, R. A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biology Letters, 4, 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0393

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, B. H., and Lindhiem, O. (2006). Carrying babies and groceries: the

  • effect of moral and social weight on caring. Ecological Psychology, 16, 93–111. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1802_2

  • Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2003). Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 867–874. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2302

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2004). The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, S88–S90. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0085

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment: Essays on livelihood, dwelling and skill. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2018). Back to the future with the theory of affordances. Journal of Ethnographic Theory, 8, 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1086/698358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurons. Cognitive Neuroscience & Neuropsychology, 7, 2325–2330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishak, S., Adolph, K. E., & Lin, G. C. (2008). Perceiving affordances for fitting through apertures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34, 1501–1514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (2021). Developmental variability and developmental cascades: Lessons from motor and language development in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30, 228–235. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721421993822

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, G. A. (2007). Ecological interface design for petrochemical process control: An empirical assessment. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Part A: Systems and Humans, 37, 906–920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joh, A. S., Adolph, K. E., Narayanan, P., & Dietz, V. (2007). Gauging possibilities for action based on friction underfoot. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1145–1157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1145

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalénine, S., Wamain, Y., Decroix, J., & Coello, Y. (2016). Conflict between object structural and functional affordances in peripersonal space. Cognition, 155, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.06.006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2011). Transition from crawling to walking and infants’ actions with objects and people. Child Development, 82, 1199–1209. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01595.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., Weir, A. A. S., Rutz, C., & Kacelnik, A. (2005). Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature, 433, 121. https://doi.org/10.1038/433121a

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, N. G., & Kim, H. (2017). Schizophrenia: An impairment in the capacity to perceive affordances. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1052. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01052

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, M., & Rogler, C. R. (2018). Affordances in interaction: The case of Aikido. Ecological Psychology, 30, 195–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2017.1409589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, S. L., Connor, R. C., Krutzen, M., & Allen, S. J. (2021). Cooperation-based concept formation in male bottlenose dolphins. Nature Communications, 12, 2373. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-22668-1

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kiverstein, J. D., & Rietveld, E. (2018). Reconceiving representation-hungry cognition: An ecological-enactive proposal. Adaptive Behavior, 26, 147–163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712318772778

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kiyosawa, M., Ito, E., Shirai, K., Kanekatsu, R., Miura, M., & Kiguchi, K. (1999). Cocoon spinning behavior in the Silkworm, Bombyx mori: Comparison of three strains constructing different cocoons in shape. Zoological Science, 16, 215–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konczak, J., Meeuwsen, H. J., & Cress, M. E. (1992). Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18, 691–697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubovy, M. (1988). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. Cambridge University Press.

  • Lakoff, G. (2012). Explaining embodied cognition results. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 773–785. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01222.x

  • Learmouth, A. E., Lamberth, R., & Rovee-Collier, C. K. (2005). The social context of imitation in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 297–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Librado, P., et al. (2021). The origins and spread of domestic horses from the Western Eurasian steppes. Nature, 598, 634–640. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04018-9

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, S. W., Huang, H.-H., Stahlin, K., & Galloway, J. C. (2014). Modified ride-on car for mobility and socialization: Single-case study of an infant with Down Sydrome. Pediatric Physical Therapy, 26, 418–426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lopresti-Goodman, S. M., River, A., & Dressel, C. (2012). Practicing safe text: The impact of texting on walking behavior. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 644–648. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2846

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L., & Bardone, E. (2010). Chances, affordances, and cognitive niche construction: The plasticity of environmental situatedness. International Journal of Advanced Intelligence Paradigms, 2, 235–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mantel, B., Hoppenot, P., & Colle, E. (2012). Perceiving for acting with teleoperated robots: Ecological principles to human-robot interaction design. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Part A: Systems and Humans, 42, 1460–1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mantel, B., Stoffregen, T. A., Campbell, A., & Bardy, B. G. (2015). Exploratory movement generates higher-order information that is sufficient for accurate perception of scaled egocentric distance. PLOS ONE, 10(4), Article e0120025. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120025

  • Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 361–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.13.3.361

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mark, L. S., Balliet, J. A., Craver, K. D., Douglas, S. D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecological Psychology, 2, 325–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mark, L. S., Nemeth, K., Gardner, D., Dainoff, M. J., Paasche, J., Duffy, M., & Grandt, K. (1997). Postural dynamics and the preferred critical boundary for visually guided reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1365–1379.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mark, L. S., Ye, L., & Smart, L. J. (2015). Perceiving the nesting of affordances for complex goal-directed actions. In R. Hoffman & J. Szalma (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of applied perception research (pp. 547–565). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, K. L., Johnston, L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Toward a radically embodied, embedded social psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1217–1225. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.666

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mech, L. D. (2007). Possible use of foresight, understanding, and planning by Wolves hunting Muskoxen. Arctic, 60, 145–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesgari, M., Mohajeri, K., & Azad, B. (2023). Affordances and information systems research: Taking stock and moving forward. The DATA BASE for Advances in Information Systems, 54, 29–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, C. F. (2003). Affordances: Four points of debate. Ecological Psychology, 15, 135–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, C. F., Weier, Z., & Harrison, S. J. (2007). Using Vision and Dynamic Touch to Perceive the Affordances of Tools. Perception, 36, 750–772. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5593

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. L. (2003). Differences between the organism-environment systems conceived by Lehrman and Gibson: What’s in the nest of reciprocities matters. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 349–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, K. S., Gomer, J. A., Pagano, C. C., & Moore, D. D. (2009). Perception of robot passability with direct line of sight and teleoperation. Human Factors, 51, 557–570. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720809341959

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, M. A., Stepp, N., & Turvey, M. T. (2011). Whole body lexical decision. Neuroscience Letters, 490, 126–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.041

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: principles and implications of

  • cognitive psychology. W.H Freeman, San Francisco. ISBN 9780716704775

  • Neisser, U. (1986). Nested structure in autobiographical memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 71–81). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K. M., Liu, Y.-T., & Mayer-Kress, G. (2001). Time scales in motor learning and development. Psychological Review, 108, 57–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nonaka, T. (2024). Selective engagement with specific invariants in the perception of art. Ecological Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2024.2355896

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nonaka, T., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2020). Social interaction in the emergence of toddler’s mealtime spoon use. Developmental Psychobiology, 62, 1124–1133. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21978

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1988). The Psychology of Everyday Things. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (2013). The Psychology of Everyday Things: Revised and (Expanded). Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neal, E. E., Zhou, S., Jiang, Y., Kearney, J. K., & Plumert, J. M. (2021). Let’s cross the next one: Parental scaffolding of prospective control over movement. Child Development, 92, e173–e185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, S. M., & Russell, M. K. (2017). Impact of postural stability and modality on the perception of passage and surface climbing. Ecological Psychology, 29, 54–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2017.1270153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ossmy, O., Han, D., Cheng, M., Kaplan, B. E., & Adolph, K. E. (2020). Look before you fit: The real-time planning cascade in children and adults. Journal of Developmental Child Psychology, 189, 104696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104696

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ossmy, O., Kaplan, B. E., Han, D., Xu, M., Bianco, C., Mukamel, R., & Adolph, K. E. (2022). Real-time processes in the development of action planning. Current Biology, 32, 190–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.11.018

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paltsev, Y. I., & Elner, A. M. (1967). Preparatory and compensatory period during voluntary movement in patients with involvement of the brain of different localization. Biofizika, 12, 142–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peker, A. T., Böge, V., Bailey, G., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2020). Perception of affordances in soccer: Kicking for power versus kicking for precision. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2020.1812494

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peker, A. T., & Böge, V., Bailey, G. S., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2023). Perception of higher-order affordances for kicking in soccer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 49, 623–624. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peker, A. T., Erkmen, N., Kocaoglu, Y., Bayraktar, Y., Arguz, A., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2020). Perception of affordances for vertical and horizontal jumping in children: Gymnasts versus non-athletes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2020.1775768

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, N. A., Hajnal, A., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2024). The complexity ofhead movement is correlated with learning about affordances for walking. Journal ofMotor Behavior, 56, 275–289. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2023.2293000

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pirotta, V., Owen, K., Donnelly, D., Brasier, M. J., & Harcourt, R. (2021). First evidence of bubble-net feeding and the formation of ‘super-groups’ by the east Australian population of humpback whales during their southward migration. Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems, 31, 2412–2419. 10. 1002/aqc.3621

  • Pitman, R. L., & Durban, J. W. (2012). Cooperative hunting behavior, prey selectivity and prey handling by pack ice killer whales (Orcinus orca), type B, in Antarctic Peninsula waters. Marine Mammal Science, 28, 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00453.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., & Cremer, J. F. (2004). Children’s perception of gap affordances: Bicycling across traffic filled intersections in an immersive virtual environment. Child Development, 75, 1243–1253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00736.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., & Cremer, J. F. (2007). Children’s road crossing: A window into perceptual-motor development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 255–258. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00515.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., Cremer, J. F., Recker, K. M., & Strutt, J. (2011). Changes in children’s perception-action tuning over short time scales: Bicycling across traffic-filled intersections in a virtual environment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 322–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • 1177/026461960001800103

  • Rader, N. D., & Zukow-Goldring, P4. (2012). Caregivers’ gestures direct infant attention during early word learning: The importance of dynamic synchrony. Language Sciences, 34, 559–568.

  • Raja, V., Valluri, D., Baggs, E., Chemero, A., & Anderson, M. L. (2021). The Markov blanket trick: On the scope of the free energy principle and active inference. Physics of Life Reviews, 39, 49–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2021.09.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raja, V., Baggs, E., Chemero, A., & Anderson, M. L. (2022). The Emperor has no blanket! Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, 48–49. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X22000243,e204

  • Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissiere, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1982). An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 98–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1989). Changing theories of postural development. In M. H. Woollacott & A. Shumway-Cook (Eds.), Development of posture and gait across the life span (pp. 1–24). University of South Carolina Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1991). Cognition as the cooperative appropriation of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 3, 135–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1996). Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S., & Bril, B. (1996). The primacy of action in development. In M. Latash and M. Turvey (Eds.), N. Bernsteins’ dexterity and its development. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410603357-17

  • Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007a). The art of conversation is coordination. Psychological Science, 18, 407–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Baron, R. (2007b). Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 845–859.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M., Shockley, K., Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behaviour. In P. Calvo & T. Gomila (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 161–187). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., Denys, D., & van Westen, M. (2018). Ecological-enactive cognition as engaging with a field of relevant affordances: The skilled intentionality framework (SIF). In A. Newen, L. De Bruin, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition (pp. 41–70). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2003). A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 26, 325–352. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2014.958035

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M. A., Wagman, J. B., Santana, M.-V., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2002). Perceptual behavior: Recurrence analysis of a haptic exploratory procedure. Perception, 31, 481–510. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3176

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rio, K. W., Dachner, G. C., & Warren, W. H. (2018). Local interactions underlyingcollective motion in human crowds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 285, 20180611. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0611

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rodger, M., Smith, O. B., & Stapleton, P. (2024). How do aesthetics get into muscles and muscles into aesthetics? Insights from musical interactions in an experimental context. Ecological Psychology, https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2024.2355881

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action, and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027839

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rottschaefer, W. A. (2021). Affording affordance moral realism. Biological Theory, 16, 30–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-020-00361-8

  • Rovee-Collier, C. K., & Cuevas, K. (2009). Multiple memory systems are unnecessary to account for infant memory development: An ecological model. Developmental Psychology, 45, 160–174.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Runeson, S. (1977). On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 18, 172–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00274.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, J. T. (1997). An ontology of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 9, 97–112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0901_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, J. T. (1999). Affordances: An ecological approach to first philosophy. In G. Weiss & H. F. Haber (Eds.), Perspectives on Embodiment: The Intersections of Nature and Culture (pp. 121–141). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarantino, A. (2003). Affordances explained. Philosophy of Science, 70, 949–961. https://doi.org/10.1086/377380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seifert, L., Dicks, M., Wittmann, F., & Wolf, P. (2021). The perception of nested affordances: An examination of expert climbers. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 52, 101843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seifert, L., Orth, D., Button, C., Brymer, E., Davids, K. (2017). An Ecological Dynamics Framework for the Acquisition of Perceptual–Motor Skills in Climbing. In: Feletti, F. (eds) Extreme Sports Medicine (pp. 365-382). Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28265-7_28

  • Shaw, R. E., Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., & Mace, W. M. (2019). Affordance types and affordance tokens: Are Gibson’s affordances trustworthy? Ecological Psychology, 31, 49–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, R., & Turvey, M. T. (1981). Coalitions as models for Ecosystems: A Realist Perspective on Perceptual Organization. In M. Kubovy & J. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual Organization (pp. 343–415). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 404–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). The link between action production and processing in infancy. In F. Grammont, D. Legrand, & P. Livet (Eds.), Naturalizing Intention in Action (pp. 67–89). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1980). Mind-brain interaction: Mentalism yes; dualism no. Neuroscience, 5, 195–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger, J. (2018). The objectivity of subjective Bayesianism. European Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 8, 539–558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-018-0200-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, N. A., Salmon, P. M., Walker, G. H., & Jenkins, D. P. (2017). Cognitive work analysis: Applications, extensions, and future directions. CRC Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, D. G., Boncoddo, R. A., Magnuson, J. S., & Dixon, J. A. (2009). The dynamics of insight: Mathematical discovery as a phase transition. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1132–1149. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.8.1132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, D. G., & Dixon, J. A. (2009). The self-organization of insight: Entropy and power laws in problem solving. Journal of Problem Solving, 2, 72–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, D. G., Dixon, J. A., & Isenhower, R. W. (2009). Dynamics of representational change: Entropy, action, and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 1811–1832. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2003). Affordances as properties of the animal-environment system. Ecological Psychology, 15, 115–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2004). Breadth and limits of the affordance concept. Ecological Psychology, 16, 79–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2020). The use and uses of depiction. In J. B. Wagman & J. J. C. Blau (Eds.), Perception as the Detection of Information: Reflections on Gibson’s Ecological Approach to Visual Perception (pp. 255–273). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Bardy, B. G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T., & Heath, R. (2022). Against free energy, for direct perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45(E212), 59–60. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X22000103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A., Mantel, B., & Bardy, B. G. (2017). The senses considered as one perceptual system. Ecological Psychology, 29, 165–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Riccio, G. E. (1988). An ecological theory of orientation and the vestibular system. Psychological Review, 95, 3–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C.-M., & Bardy, B. G. (2005). Affordance judgments and nonlocomotor body movement. Ecological Psychology, 17, 75–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szokolszky, A., Carello, C., Paltinus, Z., & Read, C. (2023). Claudia Carello—Interivew and reflection. In A. Szokolszky, C. Read, and Z. Palatinus (Eds.), Intellectural Journeys in Ecological Psychology, pp. 221-249). New York: Routledge. ISBN: 978-1-003-16072-4

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Tafuro, L. (2013). From action to interaction: Infant Object Exploration and Mothers’ Contingent Responsiveness (2013). IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5, 202–209. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAMD.2013.2269905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, B. J., Hawkins, M. M., & Nalepka, P. (2018). Perceiver as polar planimeter: Direct perception of jumping, reaching, and jump- reaching affordances for the self and others. Psychological Research, 82, 665–674. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0858-6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, B. J., & Riley, M. A. (2014). Remembered affordances reflect the fundamentally action- relevant, context-specific nature of visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2361–2371. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000015

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, B. J., Riley, M. A., & Wagman, J. B. (2020). Information and its detection: The consequences of Gibson’s theory of information pick up. In J. B. Wagman & J. J. C. Blau (Eds.), Perception as the Detection of Information: Reflections on Gibson’s Ecological Approach to Visual Perception (pp. 237–252). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, B. J., Wagman, J. B., Hawkins, M., Havens, M., & Riley, M. A. (2017). The independent perceptual calibration of action-neutral and -referential environmental properties. Perception, 46, 586–604. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616679172

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, M. A., & Tamir, D. I. (2024). Neural representations of situations and mental states are composed of sums of representations of the actions they afford. Nature Communications, 15, 620. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-44870-7

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tomono, T., Makino, R., Furuyama, N., & Mishima, H. (2019). How does a walker pass between two people standing in different configurations? Influence of personal space on aperture passing methods. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 2651. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02651

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Totterdell, J. A., Wellard, R., Reeves, I. M., Elsdon, B., Markovic, P., Yoshida, M., Fairchild, A., Sharp, G., Pitman, R. L. (2022). The first three records of killer whales (Orcinus orca) killing and eating blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus). Marine Mammal Science, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12906

  • Turvey, M. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology, 4, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (2013). Ecological perspective on perception-action: What kind of science does it entail? In Action science (Ed. Prinz et al.)

  • Turvey, M. T. (2015). Quantum-like issues at Nature’s ecological scale (the scale of organisms and their environments). Mind & Matter, 13, 7–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (2018). Lectures on perception: An ecological perspective. Routledge.

  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1981). Cognition: The view from ecological realism. Cognition, 10, 313–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(81)90063-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R. E., Reed, E. S., & Mace, W. M. (1981). Ecological laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn (1981). Cognition, 9, 237–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T., & Sheya, A. (2017). Non-obvious influences on perception-action abilities. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 1597–1603. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1223-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uchiyama, I., Anderson, D. I., Campos, J. J., Witherington, D., Frankel, C. B., Lejeune, L., & Barbu-Roth, M. (2008). Locomotor experience affects self and emotion. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1225–1241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Rooij, I., Bongers, R. M., & Haselager, F. G. (2002). A non-representational approach to imagined action. Cognitive Science, 26(3), 345–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicente, K. (1999). Cognitive work analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vicente, K., & Rasmussen, J. (1990). The ecology of human-machine systems II: Mediating “direct perception” in complex work domains. Ecological Psychology, 2, 207–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicente, K. J., & Wang, J. H. (1998). An ecological theory of expertise effects in memory recall. Psychological Review, 105, 35–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Blau, J. J. C., & Duffrin, T. (2023). Perception by effortful touch and a lawful approach to (the evolution of) perceiving and acting. In E. Bruner (Ed.), Cognitive Archaeology, Body Cognition, and the Evolution of Visuospatial Perception (pp. 25–51). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Caputo, S. E., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2016). Hierarchical nesting of affordances in a tool use task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 42(10), 1627–1642. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Caputo, S. E., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2016). Sensitivity to hierarchical relations among affordances in the assembly of asymmetric tools. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2923���2933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326969ECO1303_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 175–186. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.9.3.175

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Cialdella, V. T., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2019a). Nested affordances for reaching: Perception and performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1200–1211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Day, B. M. (2022). Tool use as detection and exploitation of information in an ecological niche. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 134, 10452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Hajnal, A. (2014). Task-specificity and anatomical independence in perception of surface properties by means of a wielded object. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 40, 2372–2391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Hajnal, A. (2016). Use your head! Perception of action possibilities by means of an object attached to the head. Experimental Brain Research., 243, 829–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Lozano, S., Jiménez, A., Covarrubias, P., & Cabrera, F. (2019b). Perception of affordances in the animal kingdom and beyond. In I. Zepeda, J. Camacho, & E. Camacho (Eds.), Aproximaciones al estudio del comportamiento y sus aplicaciones (Vol. II, pp. 70–108). Universidad de Guadalajara.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Miller, D. B. (2003). Nested reciprocities: The organism-environment system in perception-action and development. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 317–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Morgan, L. L. (2010). Nested prospectivity in perception: Perceived maximum reaching height reflects anticipated changes in reaching ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 905–909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Smith, P. J. K. (2018). Perception of affordances for stepping over an expanse with crutches. Perception, 47, 1106–1109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006618802508

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2020). It doesn’t add up: Nested affor- dances for reaching are perceived as a complex particular. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(8), 3832–3841. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02108-w

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Thomas, B. J., & McBride, D. M. (2019c). Perceiving and remembering affordances for others are continuous processes. Experimental Psychology, 65, 385–392. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000424

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., Thomas, B. J., McBride, D. M., & Day, B. M. (2013). Perception of maximum reaching height when the means of reaching are no longer in view. Ecological Psychology, 25, 63–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2013.753810

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, H. J., Li, R., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2019). Adaptive perception of changes in affordances for walking on a ship at sea. Human Movement Science, 64, 28–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, H., Peterson, N., Li, R., Wagman, J. B., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2019). Sensitivity to changes in dynamic affordances for walking on land, and at sea. PLoS ONE, 14(10), e0221974. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221974

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, H., Wagman, J. B., Stergiou, N., Erkmen, N., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2017). Dynamic perception of dynamic affordances: Walking on a ship at sea. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 517–524. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4810-6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W. H., Jr. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.683

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W. H. (2005). Direct perception: The view from here. Philosophical Topics, 33, 335–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, H. (2024). Sperm whale clans and human societies. Royal Society Open Science, 11, 231353. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.231353

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. D., & Golonka, S. (2013). Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 58. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wimpenny, J. H., Weir, A. A. S., & Kacelnik, A. (2011). New Caledonian crows use tools for non-foraging activities. Animal Cognition, 14, 459–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0366-1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, T. F., & Dahlin, C. R. (2018). Vocal dialects in parrots: patterns and processes of cultural evolution. Emu-Austral Ornithology, 118, 50–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/01584197.2017.1379356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, H., Sato, A., & Itakura, S. (2020). Transition from crawling to walking changes gaze communication space in everyday infant-parent interaction. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2987. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02987

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ye, L., Cardwell, W., & Mark, L. S. (2009). Perceiving multiple affordances for objects. Ecological Psychology, 21, 185–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407410903058229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zukow-Goldring, P. (2012). Assisted imitation: First steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34, 569–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Lois Yellowthunder for helping to illuminate theoretical issues, and for introduction to concepts and literature in anthropology. We also thank the late Michael Turvey and the late Eleanor Gibson for several decades of formal and informal mentoring, and for the example they set through their principled search for an internally consistent, general account of perceiving, cognizing, and acting. Finally, we thank Kim Vicente, whose work with the Abstraction Hierarchy was an important inspiration for our analysis.

Funding

Not applicable (none).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Both authors contributed equally to all sections and stages of manuscript preparation.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas A. Stoffregen.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethics approval

Not applicable.

Consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Both authors have given their consent for publication.

Open practices statement

None of the data or materials for the experiments reported here is available, and none of the experiments was preregistered.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stoffregen, T.A., Wagman, J.B. Higher order affordances. Psychon Bull Rev (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02535-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02535-y

Keywords

Navigation