Next Article in Journal
Personality Traits and Family SES Moderate the Relationship between Media Multitasking and Reasoning Performance
Previous Article in Journal
Mapping the Memory Structure of High-Knowledge Students: A Longitudinal Semantic Network Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Social, Demographic, and Psychological Factors Associated with Middle-Aged Mother’s Vocabulary: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study

1
Independent Scholar, London, UK
2
Department of Leadership and Organisational Behaviour, Norwegian Business School, Nydalsveien 37, 0484 Oslo, Norway
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2024, 12(6), 57; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12060057
Submission received: 6 April 2024 / Revised: 27 May 2024 / Accepted: 30 May 2024 / Published: 31 May 2024

Abstract

:
Based on a sample of 8271 mothers, this study explored a set of psychological and sociodemographic factors associated with their vocabulary, drawing on data from a large, nationally representative sample of children born in 2000. The dependent variable was maternal vocabulary assessed when cohort members were at fourteen years of age, and the mothers were in their mid-forties. Data were also collected when cohort members were at birth, 9 months old, and at ages 3, 7, 11 and 14 years. Correlational analysis showed that family income at birth, parent–child relationship quality at age 3, maternal educational qualifications at age 11, and maternal personality trait Openness at age 14 were significantly and positively associated with maternal vocabulary. It also showed maternal malaise at 9 months and children’s behavioral adjustment at age 7, and maternal traits Neuroticism and Agreeableness at age 14 were significantly and negatively associated with maternal vocabulary. Maternal age was also significantly and positively associated with vocabulary. Regression analysis showed that maternal age, malaise, parent–child relationship quality, children’s behavioral adjustment, maternal educational qualifications, and traits Openness and Agreeableness were significant predictors of maternal vocabulary, accounting for 33% of total variance. The implications and limitations are discussed.

1. Introduction

There are three particular features of this study, which used middle-aged mothers’ vocabulary score as the dependent variable. The first is the question: “what is the relationship between personality and intelligence”, which has been explored using different populations and tests of both factors. We attempted to test two theories (compensation vs. investment) using these data. The second is the relationship between social class, age, and crystallized intelligence as measured by the vocabulary test. The research suggests that there should be a clear correlation between parental social class and intelligence as measured by vocabulary such that higher social class participants would have higher scores (Hanscombe et al. 2012). There would also be a positive age effect as vocabulary, unlike fluid intelligence, increases with age, even in late middle age. Third, this study employed various measures of the parent–child relationship between seven and ten years before the test was administered. The difficulties that arise from these factors influencing parental malaise would influence the mother’s lifestyle and happiness and would be less conducive to activities like reading and social interaction, which would improve vocabulary.
In this study, we chose a number of variables from this publicly available dataset to examine the extent to which variance in IQ, as measured by the proxy variable of vocabulary, could be explained by various psychological and sociodemographic factors. Inevitably, there were other variables that we would like to have examined, but which were not available in the dataset.

2. Personality and Intelligence

There is an extensive literature on the relationship between personality and intelligence (Bédard and Le Corff 2020; Kretzschmar et al. 2018; Ziegler et al. 2012). Researchers have developed theories as to why certain traits (e.g., Conscientiousness, Openness) should be, and are, related to intelligence (Chamorro-Premuzic et al. 2004; Rammstedt et al. 2018). They are essentially developmental theories and suggest that in adulthood, different traits (Conscientiousness, Openness) would be most strongly related to cognitive ability scores, particularly crystallized intelligence.
One of the most-explored ideas is Compensation theory, which argues that Conscientiousness acts as a “coping/reimbursing strategy” for less intelligent but ambitious and competitive people. It is a way of coping in a competitive environment; those who are less intelligent have to work harder than those who are brighter to achieve the same results. Thus, relatively less intelligent individuals may become more methodical, organized, thorough, and persistent (i.e., Conscientious) to compensate for their relative lack of intelligence, particularly in a highly competitive educational or work environment; that is, they can achieve as much as bright people by simply working harder. Alternatively, relatively more intelligent people may tend to get by on their cognitive efficiency rather than strenuous effort or persistent organization. Therefore, IQ and Conscientiousness are negatively correlated. Theoretically this correlation should be less negative for measures of crystallized rather than fluid intelligence given the importance of effort in achieving the former. Various studies have investigated this hypothesis (Moutafi et al. 2004; Murray et al. 2014; Wood and Englert 2009).
Others have argued that Openness is, and should be, most closely related to intelligence. DeYoung et al. (2014) hypothesize and found that only aspects from the Openness-to-Experience domain should be empirically associated with intelligence, with the Intellect facet being more strongly associated with intelligence than its counterpart aspect, Openness. This study was replicated by Bédard and Le Corff (2020), who found that the Intellect facet was independently associated with g, verbal, and nonverbal intelligence, while its domain Openness was independently related to verbal intelligence only.
von Stumm (2018) proposed an investment theory of adult intelligence, which posits that individual differences in knowledge attainment results from people’s differences in cognitive ability and their propensity to apply and invest that ability. These she refers to as investment personality traits. von Stumm and Ackerman (2013) identified 34 trait constructs and corresponding scales that refer to intellectual investment which were classified into different trait categories.
It should be noted that there are also reasons to suspect two other traits related to intelligence scores (Cuppello et al. 2023a, 2023b). It has been suggested that those with higher Neuroticism scores perform less well in all assessments due to test anxiety, which results in a negative correlation. Similarly, extroverts perform less well than introverts in high school and university and thus at a number of IQ tests (Furnham 2023).

3. Age, Class, Education and Intelligence

There is a large literature which suggests that social class, measured by parental jobs and income, is related to factors including education and intelligence such that higher social class individuals tend to be more intelligent and more successful in educational settings (Cheng and Furnham 2014; Manstead 2018). Whilst there are a number of different and contested reasons for this, many studies have confirmed this relationship (Deary et al. 2005; Marmot 2007). There are also established age effects such that while fluid intelligence peaks around the early twenties and declines after forty, crystallized intelligence continues to grow until late middle age (Cattell 1963).

4. Depression and Malaise

There is an extensive literature on the relationship between mental health and intelligence, particularly among those who score very high or low on intelligence tests. Researchers have been interested in particular mental illnesses (Navrady et al. 2017). One issue concerns whether some illnesses are more associated with test-taking behavior, which effects scores more than actual IQ (Moutafi et al. 2006). There is a literature on the relationship between happiness/depression and IQ (Ali et al. 2013), though all researchers acknowledge the complexity of the mechanisms which leads to these results. For instance, do less intelligent people have less healthy lifestyles, or do life circumstances lead to poorer mental health which effects education and IQ development. In this study, we have employed various measures of mental health including malaise and Neuroticism.
Previous studies have shown the strong links between socioeconomic disadvantages and health (Marmot 2004, 2007, 2010; Wilkinson and Marmot 2003) and maternal depression (Brown and Harris 1978; Kiernan and Huerta 2008). Maternal depression is linked with parenting (Kohlhoff and Barnett 2013; Turney 2011) and children’s behavioral problems (Fihrer et al. 2009; Herba 2014; Hoffman et al. 2006; Kuckertz et al. 2018).

4.1. This Study

This study explores a set of psychological and sociodemographic factors associated with maternal vocabulary, drawing on data collected from a large nationally representative sample born in 2000 in the UK, the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). Vocabulary knowledge is considered to be part of verbal IQ, which is closely related to total IQ (Smith et al. 2005). In the current study, we examine family income, maternal mental health, parent–child relationship, children’s behavioral problems, maternal education, and the Big-Five personality traits in relation to maternal cognitive assessment (i.e., vocabulary). In this sense, we test many hypotheses based on previous psychological and sociological studies concerned with personal and demographic correlates of IQ.

4.2. Hypotheses

Based on the literature reviewed above five, hypotheses were formulated. (H1) Family income would be significantly and positively associated with maternal vocabulary; (H2) maternal malaise would be significantly and negatively associated with maternal vocabulary; (H3) parent–child relationship quality would be significantly and positively associated with maternal vocabulary; (H4) children’s behavioral problems would be significantly and negatively associated with maternal vocabulary; (H5) maternal education would be significantly and positively associated with maternal vocabulary; (H6) maternal traits Openness (positively), Extroversion, Conscientiousness, and Neuroticism (negatively) would be significantly associated with maternal vocabulary; (H7) Family income, maternal malaise, parent–child relationship quality, children’s behavioral problems, maternal education, and maternal traits openness and emotional stability would all be significant predictors of the outcome variable.

5. Method

5.1. Sample

The study draws on data collected for the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS, https://www.data-archive.ac.uk/), a survey of 18,818 babies born between September 2000 and January 2002 into 18,552 families living in the UK (Dex and Joshi 2005). The first sweep of the Millennium Cohort Study was carried out during 2001 and 2002, when most babies were 9-months old. Data were collected from the parents of the babies via personal interviews and self-completion questionnaires. The sample design allowed for disproportionate representation of families living in areas of child poverty. Due to disproportionate sampling, special weights have to be applied in analyzing the data (Plewis et al. 2004). The families were followed up. The following analyses are based on data from surveyr at 9 months of age and 3, 7, 11, and 14 years. Data were collected from parents (mainly mothers) via personal interview sand self-completion questionnaires. In 2015, when cohort members were at 14 years of age, 11,872 cohort members from 11,726 families took part in the survey (response = 76%). A total of 10,282 mothers completed a vocabulary assessment. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study (Shepherd and Gilbert 2019). The following analyses are based on 8271 mothers and their children, for whom we have complete data. In comparison to the original sample, the analytic sample contains relatively more socially privileged and better-educated mothers, and slightly more boys.

5.2. Measures

  • Maternal age: Information on maternal age were provided by mothers through interviews in several sweeps. The maternal age when cohort members were born was used in the study. The mean age was 29.65 (SD = 5.38), ranging from 14 to 47 years old. In effect, they were around 43 years old when the study was run.
  • Family income: Family income of the household was reported at birth. The logged family income is used for the analyses.
  • Maternal malaise: This was assessed when cohort members were 9 months old. A shortened 9-item version of the Rutter Malaise Inventory (Rutter et al. 1970) was used, which is a self-completion instrument measuring depression, anxiety, and psychosomatic illness (Rutter et al. 1970), and it correlates significantly with previously diagnosed and currently treated depression. It has been shown to be relatively stable over time (Furnham and Cheng 2015). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.75.
  • Parent–child relationship: This was assessed when cohort members were at 3 years of age using the Pianta scale (Pianta 1992), comprising 15 items on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = definitely not apply; 5 = definitely applies). Example items: “I share an affectionate, warm relationship with my child”; “dealing with my child drains my energy”. Information was collected at age 3 using the mother’s report. Responses were summed, with a high score indicating a better parent–child relationship. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.77.
  • Children’s behavioral problems: This was measured with the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) via the mother’s report. Mothers were interviewed when cohort members were at 7 years of age. Behavioral adjustment at age 7 is a behavioral screening questionnaire for 3-to-16 year olds (Goodman and Goodman 2009; Goodman 1997, 2001). It consists of 25 items, assessed via parental report, generating scores for five subscales measuring hyperactivity, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, peer problems, and prosocial behavior. The total difficulties score does not incorporate the prosocial scale, which measures prosocial behavior (Goodman 1997). Thus, the four behavioral problems subscales were used as the outcome measures. Each SDQ item has three possible answers, which are assigned a value: 0 = not true; 1 = somewhat true; or 2 = certainly true. The 20-item SDQ total score was used in the following analysis. Cronbach’s alpha for hyperactivity was 0.79; for emotional symptoms, it was 0.66; for conduct problems, it was 0.62; and for peer problems, it was 0.61. Cronbach’s alpha for the SDQ total score was 0.73.
  • Maternal education: This was measured when cohort members were at age 11. Mothers were asked about their highest academic or vocational qualifications. Responses are coded according to the six-point scale of National Vocational Qualifications levels (NVQ), which ranges from “none” to “university degree/higher”/equivalent NVQ 5 or 6.
  • Maternal personality traits: The Big-Five personality traits of Extroversion, Neuroticism/Emotional Stability, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness were assessed when cohort members were at age 14 years. We used a shortened version (3 items for each trait) of the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) (Goldberg 1999), a self-completion questionnaire. Responses (7-point scale: 1 is “does not apply to me at all” and 7 is “applies to me perfectly”). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.60 for Extroversion, 0.67 for Neuroticism/Emotional Stability, 0.53 for Agreeableness, 0.52 for Conscientiousness, and 0.66 for Openness.
  • Maternal vocabulary: This was assessed when cohort members were at 14 years of age. It is a word activity assessment measuring knowledge of vocabulary. Mothers were asked to complete the word activity during the interviewer visit. It measures respondents’ understanding of the meaning of words. The assessment involved presenting the respondent with a list of target words, each of which had five other words next to them. The respondent had to select, from the five options, the word which meant the same, or nearly the same, as the target word (i.e., synonyms). Each respondent had a list of 20 target words. The assessment was carried out on the interviewer’s tablet.

6. Results

6.1. Correlational Analysis

Table 1 shows the correlations and the means and SDs of all variables in this study. Maternal vocabulary was significantly and positively associated with family income, parent–child relationship quality, maternal education, and maternal trait Openness, and significantly and negatively associated with maternal malaise, children’s behavioral problems, and trait Neuroticism (p < .05 to p < .001). Thus, (H1) to (H6) were supported. Maternal age at birth was also significantly and positively associated with maternal vocabulary. Among the Big-Five personality traits, Extroversion and Conscientiousness were not related to maternal vocabulary (omitted in further analysis). The strongest associations were between maternal education and maternal vocabulary (r = 0.46; p < .001), followed by maternal age and family income (r = 0.39 and r = 0.37, respectively; p < .001).

6.2. Regressional Analysis

Table 2 shows the results of the regressional analysis. In Model 1, all six psychological variables—maternal malaise, parent–child relationship quality, children’s behavioral problems, and traits Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Openness—were significant predictors of maternal vocabulary (Beta ranged from −0.03 to 0.17; p < .05 to p < .001), accounting for 7% of the variance. In Model 2, after entering the three sociodemographic variables into the equation, trait Neuroticism ceased to be a significant predictor of the outcome variable, whilst maternal age, family income, and maternal education (Beta ranged from 0.13 to 0.35; p < .001) accounted for 26% of variance, thus explaining 33% of the total variance.

7. Discussion

This study confirmed and extended the previous findings in the area. Among the psychological factors, the strongest predictor of maternal vocabulary was trait Openness (Beta = 0.10; p < .001). This is in line with previous findings which have shown the significant and positive association between Openness and numerous measures of intelligence (Furnham 2008). Indeed, it supports the investment hypothesis, which would suggest that a measure of vocabulary would be particularly affected by an investment into learning through consumption of various media and social interactions. Open people are curious and interested in new ideas and new knowledge, which inevitably leads to greater knowledge (crystallized intelligence).
The associations between Conscientiousness and cognitive ability and between Agreeableness and cognitive ability are not unequivocal in previous studies. Correlation coefficients are usually very modest. In the current study, Conscientiousness was not associated with maternal vocabulary (r = 0.01, ns), though previous findings have shown this trait to be significantly and positively association with occupational attainment, which is largely related to cognitive ability (Furnham 2008). One of the explanations of the association between Conscientiousness and cognitive ability might be due to some facets of Conscientiousness (MacCann et al. 2009) that affect vocabulary assessment, which have a time restriction. This, therefore, does not provide support for the Compensation theory.
In the current study, Agreeableness was significantly and negatively associated with maternal vocabulary (Beta = −0.06; p < .001). It is not clear why agreeable mothers tend to have lower scores on vocabulary assessment, though it could be related to a preference for particular reading material or social interaction patterns.
The current study also showed that maternal malaise and parent–child relationship quality were modestly but significantly associated with maternal vocabulary. Intelligent mothers tend to be less prone to mental health problems and are more likely to have good relationships with their children. It might be that intelligent mothers tend to have an authoritative (rather than authoritarian or permissive) rearing style in their children’s upbringing and to nurture children’s behavioral adjustment.
There was a significant and positive association between maternal age when cohort members were born and maternal vocabulary, suggesting that intelligent mothers tend to have children relatively later in their lives. Indeed, we know that vocabulary is related to age; therefore, this may simply be evidence of an age effect.
Mothers’ age was significantly and positively associated with family income (r = 0.39, p < .00), and more mature mothers are more able to provide their children with better living standards and learning facilities compared with younger mothers, especially teenager mothers. There are data to suggest that social class is related to the age of marriage, with lower SES people being associated with earlier marriages (Karney 2021).
Educational achievement was the strongest predictor of maternal vocabulary (Beta = 0.35; p < .001). The association might be bi-directional: intelligent children are more likely to obtain higher educational qualifications, and educational qualifications lead to higher occupational attainment in a more mentally stimulating working environment, which would help maintain individuals’ cognitive abilities.

Limitations

Whilst it is very desirable to be able to explore a dataset such as this, it is also not always clear as to the causal relationships between the variables. Thus, our major variable of interest, namely, vocabulary (acting as a proxy for IQ), could be understood as either or both an outcome or a predictor variable. Thus, trait Openness could be seen to lead to a higher vocabulary because of curiosity; but equally, a greater vocabulary could increase Openness because of wider access to new and stimulating material. Similarly, in our regressions, we used vocabulary as our independent (criterion) variable, and factors such as malaise as dependent (predictor) variables, but we could have reversed this by looking at the effect of vocabulary and other factors on malaise.
As with all research using cohort studies, the variables used in the study are constrained by the availability of the data. Many of the measures—for instance, those of personality—were very short, which could threaten their validity. Another limitation is the attrition of respondents over time. Since sample attrition is greatest amongst individuals in more deprived circumstances, our results may thus be a conservative estimate. Another limitation is that all measurements were made via mother-report, with the concomitant problems of method invariance.
Further, it would be desirable to have maternal cognitive ability assessed earlier, i.e., at the birth of cohort members. To build a robust causal model of the participants’ IQ (vocabulary) in their mid-forties, it would have been desirable to have more data on such things as the number of children they had, family dynamics, and partner support, as well as occupational status history and other personality and motivational variables like ambition and tolerance for ambiguity (Cuppello et al. 2023a, 2023b).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.C. and A.F. conceived and designed the study; formal analysis, H.C.; investigation, H.C.; writing—original draft preparation, H.C. and A.F.; writing—review and editing, A.F.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Multi Research Ethics Committee (MREC) has been sought for all MCS follow-ups (Shepherd and Gilbert 2019).

Data Availability Statement

Further details of the data available from the main survey are available from the CLS website and, in particular, the MCS Guide to the Datasets at www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/mcs, accessed on 5 April 2024.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ali, Afia, Gareth Ambler, Andre Strydom, Dheeraj Rai, Claudia Cooper, Sally McManus, Scott Weich, Howard Meltzer, Simon Dein, and Angela Hassiotis. 2013. The relationship between happiness and intelligent quotient: The contribution of socio-economic and clinical factors. Psychological Medicine 43: 1303–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bédard, Marc-André, and Yann Le Corff. 2020. Intelligence and personality: A replication and extension study of the association between intelligence and personality aspects. Journal of Individual Differences 41: 124–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Brown, George. W., and Tirril Harris. 1978. Social Origins of Depression. London: Tavistock. [Google Scholar]
  4. Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas, Adrian Furnham, and Joanna Moutafi. 2004. The relationship between estimated and psychometric personality and intelligence scores. Journal of Research in Personality 38: 505–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cheng, Helen, and Adrian Furnham. 2014. The Associations Between Parental Socio-Economic Conditions, Childhood Intelligence, Adult Personality Traits, Social Status and Mental Well-Being. Social Indicators Research 117: 653–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Cuppello, Stephen, Luke Treglown, and Adrian Furnham. 2023a. Intelligence, Personality and Tolerance of Ambiguity. Journal of Intelligence 11: 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cuppello, Stephen, Luke Treglown, and Adrian Furnham. 2023b. Personality and Management Level: Traits that get you to the top. Personality and Individual Difference 206: 11211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Deary, Ian J., Michelle D. Taylor, Carole L. Hart, Valerie Wilson, George Davey Smith, David Blane, and John M. Starr. 2005. Intergenerational social mobility and mid-life status attainment: Influences of childhood intelligence, childhood social factors, and education. Intelligence 33: 455–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Dex, Shirley, and Heather Joshi. 2005. Children of the 21st Century. From Birth to Nine Months. Bristol: Policy Press. [Google Scholar]
  11. DeYoung, Colin G., Lena C. Quilty, Jordan B. Peterson, and Jeremy R. Gray. 2014. Openness to experience, intellect, and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality Assessment 96: 46–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fihrer, Irene, Catherine A. McMahon, and Alan J. Taylor. 2009. The impact of postnatal and concurrent maternal depression on child behaviour during the early school years. Journal of Affective Disorders 119: 116–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Furnham, Adrian. 2008. Personality and Intelligence at Work. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  14. Furnham, Adrian. 2023. Personality and Intelligence: Compensation and Investment. Psychology 14: 1651–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Furnham, Adrian, and Helen Cheng. 2015. The stability and change of malaise scores over 27 years: Findings from a nationally representative sample. Personality and Individual Differences 79: 30–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Goldberg, Lewis R. 1999. A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower level facets of several five-factor models. In Personality Psychology in Europe. Edited by I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt and F. Ostendorf. Tilburg: Tilburg University Press, vol. 7, pp. 7–28. [Google Scholar]
  17. Goodman, Anna, and Robert Goodman. 2009. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as Dimensional Measure of Child Mental Health. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48: 400–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Goodman, Robert. 1997. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38: 581–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Goodman, Robert. 2001. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40: 1337–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Hanscombe, Ken B., Maciej Trzaskowski, Claire M. A. Haworth, Oliver S. P. Davis, Philip S. Dale, and Robert Plomin. 2012. Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Children’s Intelligence (IQ): In a UKRepresentative Sample SES Moderates the Environmental, Not Genetic, Effect on IQ. PLoS ONE 7: e30320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Herba, Catherine M. 2014. Maternal depression and child behavioural outcomes. Lancet Psychiatry 1: 408–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hoffman, Casey, Keith A. Crnic, and Jason K. Baker. 2006. Maternal depression and parenting: Implications for children’s emergent emotion regulation and behavioral functioning. Parenting: Science and Practice 6: 271–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Karney, Benjamin R. 2021. Socioeconomic status and intimate relationships. Annual Review of Psychology 72: 391–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kiernan, Kathleen E., and M. Carmen Huerta. 2008. Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. British Journal of Sociology 59: 783–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kohlhoff, Jane, and Bryanne Barnett. 2013. Parenting self-efficacy: Links with maternal depression, infant behaviour and adult attachment. Early Human Development 89: 249–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kretzschmar, André, Marion Spengler, Anna-Lena Schubert, Ricarda Steinmayr, and Matthias Ziegler. 2018. The Relation of Personality and Intelligence—What Can the Brunswick Symmetry Principle Tell Us? Journal of Intelligence 6: 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kuckertz, Jennie M., Colter Mitchell, and Jillian Lee Wiggins. 2018. Parenting mediates the impact of maternal depression on child internalizing symptoms. Depression and Anxiety 35: 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. MacCann, Carolyn, Angela Lee Duckworth, and Richard D. Roberts. 2009. Empirical identification of the major facets of conscientiousness. Learning and Individual Differences 19: 451–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Manstead, Antony S. R. 2018. The psychology of social class: How socioeconomic status impacts thought, feelings, and behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology 57: 267–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Marmot, Michael. 2004. Status Syndrome. London: Bloomsbury. [Google Scholar]
  31. Marmot, Michael. 2007. Achieving health equity: From root causes to fair outcomes. Lancet 370: 1153–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Marmot, Michael. 2010. Fair Society Healthy Lives. The Marmot Review: Strategic Review of Health Inequalities in England Post. London: Fair Society Healthy Lives (The Marmot Review)—IHE. [Google Scholar]
  33. Moutafi, Joanna, Adrian Furnham, and Ioannis Tsaousis. 2006. Is the relationship between intelligence and trait Neuroticism mediated by test anxiety? Personality and Individual Differences 40: 587–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Moutafi, Joanna, Adrian Furnham, and Laurence Paltiel. 2004. Why is Conscientiousness negatively correlated with intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences 37: 1013–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Murray, Aja L., Wendy Johnson, Matt McGue, and William G. Iacono. 2014. How are conscientiousness and cognitive ability related to one another? A re-examination of the intelligence compensation hypothesis. Personality and Individual Differences 70: 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Navrady, L. B., S. J. Ritchie, S. W. Y. Chan, D. M. Kerr, M. J. Adams, E. H. Hawkins, D. Porteous, I. J. Deary, C. R. Gale, G. D. Batty, and et al. 2017. Intelligence and neuroticism in relation to depression and psychological distress: Evidence from two large population cohorts. European Psychiatry 43: 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Pianta, Robert C. 1992. Parent-Child Relationship Scale. Richmond: University of Virginia. [Google Scholar]
  38. Plewis, Ian, Lisa Calderwood, Denise Hawkes, Gareth Hughes, and Heather Joshi. 2004. Millennium Cohort Study. Technical Report on Sampling. London: Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. [Google Scholar]
  39. Rammstedt, Beatrice, Clemens M. Lechner, and Daniel Danner. 2018. Relationships between personality and cognitive ability: A facet-level analysis. Journal of Intelligence 6: 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rutter, M., J. Tizard, and K. Whitmore. 1970. Education, Health and Behaviour. London: Longmans. [Google Scholar]
  41. Shepherd, Peter, and Emily Gilbert. 2019. Millennium Cohort Study, Ethical Review and Consent. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University College London. Available online: www.cls.ucl.ac.uk (accessed on 1 January 2021).
  42. Smith, Billy L., Teresa D. Smith, Loria Taylor, and Melissa Hobby. 2005. Relationship between Intelligence and Vocabulary. Perceptual and Motor Skills 100: 101–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Turney, Kristin. 2011. Labored love: Examining the link between maternal depression and parenting behaviors. Social Science Research 40: 399–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. von Stumm, Sophie. 2018. Better open than intellectual: The benefits of investment personality traits for learning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 44: 562–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. von Stumm, Sophie, and Phillip Ackerman. 2013. Investment and Intellect: A Review and Metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin 139: 841–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Wilkinson, Richard G., and Michael Marmot. 2003. The Solid Facts. Copenhagen: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  47. Wood, Paul, and Paul Englert. 2009. Intelligence compensation theory: A critical examination of the negative relationship between conscientiousness and fluid and crystallised intelligence. Australasian Journal of Organisational Psychology 2: 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ziegler, Matthias, Erik Danay, Moritz Heene, Jens Asendorpf, and Markus Bühner. 2012. Openness, fluid intelligence, and crystallized intelligence: Toward an integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality 46: 173–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Pearson product–moment correlations of maternal vocabulary and other variables in the study.
Table 1. Pearson product–moment correlations of maternal vocabulary and other variables in the study.
VariablesMean
(SD)
123456789101112
1.Maternal vocabulary
at age 14
12.20
(3.96)
_
2.Maternal age
at birth
29.65
(5.38)
0.39 ***_
3.Logged family income
at birth
2.51
(0.30)
0.37 ***0.39 ***_
4.Maternal malaise
at 9 months
2.51
(0.30)
−0.10 ***−0.11 ***0.16 ***_
5.Parent–child relationship quality at age 365.01
(6.45)
0.06 ***0.11 ***0.11 ***−0.27 ***_
6.Children’s behavioral problems at age 76.59
(4.99)
−0.17 ***−0.19 ***−0.23 ***0.26 ***0.39 ***_
7.Maternal education
at age 11
3.09
(1.21)
0.46 **0.22 ***0.36 ***−0.06 ***0.06 ***−0.18 ***_
8.Maternal Extroversion
at age 14
14.42
(3.90)
−0.010.020.08 ***−0.10 ***0.08 ***−0.11 ***0.03 *_
9.Maternal Neuroticism
at age 14
11.85
(4.11)
−0.04 **−0.09 ***−0.10 ***0.32 ***0.19 ***0.21 ***−0.07 ***0.25 ***_
10.Maternal Agreeableness
at age 14
18.10
(2.63)
−0.06 ***0.01−0.02−0.05 ***0.12 ***−0.08 ***−0.010.16 ***−0.07 ***_
11.Maternal Conscientiousness
at age 14
17.66
(2.99)
0.010.04 **0.09 ***−0.13 ***0.14 ***−0.15 ***0.05 ***0.26 ***−0.15 ***0.38 ***_
12.Maternal Openness
at age 14
13.93
(3.73)
0.14 ***0.09 ***0.09 ***−0.03 *0.07 ***−0.09 ***0.16 ***0.29 ***−0.10 ***0.19 ***0.24 ***_
Note: Variables were scored such that a higher score indicated the mother’s higher age when giving birth to cohort members, a higher score on family income, higher scores on maternal malaise, higher scores on positive parent–child relationship, higher scores on children behavioral problems, higher scores on traits maternal Extroversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness, and higher scores on maternal vocabulary. Bold coefficients indicate the associations between the outcome variables and other variables examined in the study. Analysis is weighted using UK sampling weight. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.
Table 2. Predicting maternal vocabulary.
Table 2. Predicting maternal vocabulary.
MeasuresModel 1Model 2
BetatBetatp
Psychological factors
Maternal malaise−0.053.09 **−0.042.79 **.005
Parent–child relationship quality0.042.63 **0.032.72 **.007
Children’s behavioral problems−0.1611.26 ***−0.064.33 ***<.001
Neuroticism−0.032.07 *0.021.67.056
Agreeableness−0.096.54 ***−0.065.43 ***<.001
Openness0.1712.32 ***0.107.40 ***<.001
Sociodemographic factors
Maternal age 0.2520.77 ***<.001
Logged family income 0.1310.08 ***<.001
Maternal education 0.3528.95 ***<.001
Variance explainedR2 adjusted = 0.07R2 adjusted = 0.33
F = 49.02 ***F = 242.07 ***
Note. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001. † Significance levels in the final model. Analysis is weighted using UK sampling weight.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cheng, H.; Furnham, A. Social, Demographic, and Psychological Factors Associated with Middle-Aged Mother’s Vocabulary: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 57. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12060057

AMA Style

Cheng H, Furnham A. Social, Demographic, and Psychological Factors Associated with Middle-Aged Mother’s Vocabulary: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(6):57. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12060057

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cheng, Helen, and Adrian Furnham. 2024. "Social, Demographic, and Psychological Factors Associated with Middle-Aged Mother’s Vocabulary: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 6: 57. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12060057

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop