Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Prospective memory enables a person to recall an intention to do something. In that sense, while it retains information from the past, prospective memory is more future-oriented than other categories of memory.

How We Use Prospective Memory

Prospective memory, which compiles one's intentions to act in a certain way in the future, has been described as “remembering to remember.” The sorts of memories that fit into this category allow people to accomplish the tasks that they want to do or are supposed to do—but can’t do immediately.

What are some examples of prospective memory?

Prospective memory is at work when one remembers to call a friend at a specific time, take a prescribed medication every night, buy a gift for a family member’s birthday, run errands after work, vote on Election Day, and any number of things that people form an intention to do at a later point. 

Why is prospective memory important?

Without the ability to remember intentions, it would be much more challenging for people to stick to plans and meet obligations. The instances in which prospective memory falls short—when people forget to do the things they are supposed to do until it’s too late, resulting in disappointment or guilt—highlight why it is invaluable when it does work.

How Prospective Memory Works

At the center of prospective memory is an intention: to go somewhere at a certain time, to say something to a specific person, or to take any other course of action. To fulfill that intention, one may engage in periodic monitoring—for example, mentally checking on a list of errands throughout the weekend and considering whether there is time in the next hour to do one or two. But people also retrieve prospective memories spontaneously, as when someone sees a neighbor on the street and suddenly remembers wanting to ask her a question.

What parts of the brain are involved in prospective memory?

The monitoring of prospective memory intentions is supported by the anterior prefrontal cortex, research suggests. The spontaneous recollection of these intentions appears to involve the medial temporal lobe, which includes the hippocampus.

Why do we forget to do things?

A number of factors can interfere with retrieving prospective memories and acting on them. These may include setting intentions that are too vague (such as “exercise today,” as opposed to “go for a jog immediately after work is over at 6 p.m.”); insufficient use of reminders (such as notes or calendar alerts) when juggling many tasks; or interruptions that interfere with carrying out an act when an intention is recalled.