Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2010 Oct;14(10):448-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005. Epub 2010 Aug 18.

Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain

Affiliations
Review

Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain

Melissa Hines. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010 Oct.

Abstract

Male and female fetuses differ in testosterone concentrations beginning as early as week 8 of gestation. This early hormone difference exerts permanent influences on brain development and behavior. Contemporary research shows that hormones are particularly important for the development of sex-typical childhood behavior, including toy choices, which until recently were thought to result solely from sociocultural influences. Prenatal testosterone exposure also appears to influence sexual orientation and gender identity, as well as some, but not all, sex-related cognitive, motor and personality characteristics. Neural mechanisms responsible for these hormone-induced behavioral outcomes are beginning to be identified, and current evidence suggests involvement of the hypothalamus and amygdala, as well as interhemispheric connectivity, and cortical areas involved in visual processing.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Influences of prenatal androgen exposure in females, caused by congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), on preferences for sex-typed toys and on a broad measure of sex-typical activity and interest preferences. Girls with CAH are exposed to high levels of androgens prenatally, similar to levels experienced by unaffected boys and boys with CAH. For toy preferences, values are the mean (+SD) percentages of time spent playing with girls’ toys (a) or boys’ toys (b) in a playroom setting where a variety of girls’ toys (e.g., dolls, tea set, cosmetics) and boys��� toys (e.g., cars, trucks, guns) are available, along with neutral toys (e.g., books, puzzles, crayons and paper). For sex-typed activities and interests, values are mean (+SD) scores on the Pre-School Activites Inventory (PSAI), a 24-item, standardized measure . Scoring of the PSAI involves subtracting scores on girl-typical items from scores on boy-typical items. Consequently, higher scores represent more male-typical behavior. In all three panels (a – c), means for girls and boys differ significantly, as do means for girls with and without CAH. Data in panels a and b are adapted from and data in panel c are adapted from .
Figure 2
Figure 2
Examples of a male and a female vervet monkey contacting human children’s sex-typed toys. The female animal (left) appears to be inspecting the doll, in a manner similar to that in which vervet monkeys inspect infant vervets. The male animal (right) appears to be moving the car along the ground as a child might do. Reproduced with permission from .

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Darwin C. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray; 1871.
    1. Geary DC. Male, female: the evolution of human sex differences. APA Press; 2010.
    1. Laland KN, Brown G. Sense and nonsense: evolutionary perspectives on human behavior. Oxford University Press; 2002.
    1. Bussey K, Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review. 1999;106:676–713. - PubMed
    1. Martin CL, et al. Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:903–933. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources