2021
DOI: 10.1186/s13229-021-00415-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Towards robust and replicable sex differences in the intrinsic brain function of autism

Abstract: Background Marked sex differences in autism prevalence accentuate the need to understand the role of biological sex-related factors in autism. Efforts to unravel sex differences in the brain organization of autism have, however, been challenged by the limited availability of female data. Methods We addressed this gap by using a large sample of males and females with autism and neurotypical (NT) control individuals (ABIDE; Autism: 362 males, 82 fema… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
34
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 120 publications
3
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The exclusion criteria for subjects were as follows: 1) without functional or structural images; 2) without handedness information or with mixed handedness; 3) without full-scale intelligence quotient (FIQ) information or FIQ smaller than 70 22 ; 4) eye status, time of repetition (TR), slice number, or data matrix size different from those of most subjects within a site; we used 180 time points for all Stanford subjects; 5) time points different from those of most subjects within a site (at the Stanford site, there were 20 subjects with 240 time points (50%), 17 subjects had 180 time points (42.5%), one subject had 181 time points, two subjects had 238 time points, and we truncated all the functional data to 180 time points. ); 6) severe artefacts and signal losses in functional images (by visual inspection); 7) scan duration less than 100 time points 23 ; 8) head motion exceeding 3 mm or 3 degrees; 9) bad spatial normalization (by visual inspection); 10) scan cover less than 91% of the whole brain; 11) spatial correlation < 0.6 (a threshold defined by mean - 2SD) between each participant’s regional homogeneity (ReHo) map and the group mean ReHo map 24 ; 12) some subjects were excluded to ensure group matching for age, FIQ, mean framewise displacement (mFD) 25 ( p > 0.05, Two-sample t-test) and sex and handedness ( p > 0.05, Chi-square test) in each centre; 13) at each step, any sites (UCLA_2 after poor spatial normalization, Caltech after inadequate cover) with less than 20 individual datasets were excluded; 14) subjects were screened according to DCM-IV (sites with a DSM-IV score of -9999 and a certain invalid score were excluded) and age (7-25 years); ultimately, a total of 609 subjects, including 269 ASDs and 340 HCs at 13 sites, were included in our investigation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The exclusion criteria for subjects were as follows: 1) without functional or structural images; 2) without handedness information or with mixed handedness; 3) without full-scale intelligence quotient (FIQ) information or FIQ smaller than 70 22 ; 4) eye status, time of repetition (TR), slice number, or data matrix size different from those of most subjects within a site; we used 180 time points for all Stanford subjects; 5) time points different from those of most subjects within a site (at the Stanford site, there were 20 subjects with 240 time points (50%), 17 subjects had 180 time points (42.5%), one subject had 181 time points, two subjects had 238 time points, and we truncated all the functional data to 180 time points. ); 6) severe artefacts and signal losses in functional images (by visual inspection); 7) scan duration less than 100 time points 23 ; 8) head motion exceeding 3 mm or 3 degrees; 9) bad spatial normalization (by visual inspection); 10) scan cover less than 91% of the whole brain; 11) spatial correlation < 0.6 (a threshold defined by mean - 2SD) between each participant’s regional homogeneity (ReHo) map and the group mean ReHo map 24 ; 12) some subjects were excluded to ensure group matching for age, FIQ, mean framewise displacement (mFD) 25 ( p > 0.05, Two-sample t-test) and sex and handedness ( p > 0.05, Chi-square test) in each centre; 13) at each step, any sites (UCLA_2 after poor spatial normalization, Caltech after inadequate cover) with less than 20 individual datasets were excluded; 14) subjects were screened according to DCM-IV (sites with a DSM-IV score of -9999 and a certain invalid score were excluded) and age (7-25 years); ultimately, a total of 609 subjects, including 269 ASDs and 340 HCs at 13 sites, were included in our investigation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, while an open science movement with limited diversity may still provide theoretical access to open science tools (for some people), it will fail to challenge the more fundamental issue of hierarchies within the scientific process and as such limit the degree to which it is compatible with feminism. As noted above, it may also stifle efforts to improve female representation within neuroscience (Schrouff et al, 2019) which would be detrimental to scientific inquiry (Murphy et al, 2020). We therefore propose that open science groups consider diversity within the movement (as should feminist researchers; Westmarland & Bows, 2018) and this could be practically achieved through promoting the learning of and broad engagement with open science of any degree rather than exclusivity.…”
Section: Inclusivity and Accessibilitymentioning
confidence: 97%
“…It has offered novel strategies to examine robustness of methodology, design, and data which may be particularly relevant for feminists seeking to scrutinize claims about innate sex differences and dominant cultural beliefs about binary biological essentialism. Open science also presents a credible language with which to articulate frustrations about the political bias of sex difference research (as per Wickham, 2020), which may be especially important given the longstanding history of critical women's voices being marginalized in the mainstream (Murphy et al, 2020;Whitaker & Guest, 2020). However, before exploring the different tools that may provide a useful opportunity to challenge the brain sex differences evidence, we first outline the main areas of scientific concern within the research on hardwired sex brain differences.…”
Section: An Open Science Perspective On Brain Sex Difference Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations