This is the html version of the file https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9780429499821-75/unequal-childhoods-class-race-family-life-annette-lareau.
Google automatically generates html versions of documents as we crawl the web.
Page 1
444 | PaRt VIII: HoW InequaLIty SPILLS oVeR
neighborhood, and the other a nearby poor black
neighborhood. about one-half of the children are
white and about one-half are black. one child is in-
terracial. The research assistants and I carried out
individual interviews (averaging two hours each)
with all of the mothers and most of the fathers (or
guardians) of eighty-eight children, for a total of 137
interviews. We also observed children as they took
part in organized activities in the communities sur-
rounding the schools. The most intensive part of the
research, however, involved home observations of
twelve children and their families. nine of the twelve
families came from the classrooms I observed, but
the boy and girl from the two black middle-class
families and the boy from the poor white family
came from other sites. Most observations and inter-
views took place between 1993 and 1995, but inter-
views were done as early as 1990 and as late as 1997.
This chapter focuses primarily on the findings from
the observations of these twelve families since the
key themes discussed here surfaced during this part
of the fieldwork. I do include some information
from the larger study to provide a context for under-
standing the family observations. all names are
pseudonyms.
Intensive Family Observations
The research assistants and I took turns visiting the
participating families daily, for a total of about
twenty visits in each home, often in the space of one
month. The observations were not limited to the
home. Fieldworkers followed children and parents as
they took part in school activities, church services
and events, organized play, kin visits, and medical
appointments. Most field observations lasted about
three hours; sometimes, depending on the event
(e.g., an out-of-town funeral, a special extended
family event, or a long shopping trip), they lasted
much longer. In most cases, there was one overnight
visit. We often carried tape recorders with us and
used the audiotapes for reference in writing up field
notes. Families were paid $350, usually at the end of
the visits, for their participation.
A Note on Class
My purpose in undertaking the field observations
was to develop an intensive, realistic portrait of fam-
ily life. although I deliberately focused on only
twelve families, I wanted to compare children across
gender and race lines. adopting the fine-grained dif-
ferentiation of categories characteristic of current
neo-Marxist and neo-Weberian empirical studies was
not tenable. My choice of class categories was further
limited by the school populations at the sites I had
selected. Very few of the students were children of
employers or of self-employed workers. I decided to
concentrate exclusively on those whose parents were
employees. Various criteria have been proposed to
differentiate within this heterogeneous group, but
authority in the workplace and “credential barriers”
are the two most commonly used. I assigned the
families in the study to a working-class or mid-
TABLE 67.1
Argument of Unequal Childhoods: Class Differences in Childrearing
Childrearing Approach
Concerted Cultivation
Accomplishment of Natural Growth
Key Elements
Parent actively fosters and assesses child’s
Parent cares for child and allows child to grow
talents, opinions, and skills
Organization of Daily Life *multiple child leisure activities orchestrated
*child “hangs out” particularly with kin
by adults
Language Use
*reasoning/directives
*directives
*child contestation of adult statements
*rare for child to question or challenge adults
*extended negotiations between parents
*general acceptance by child of directives
and child
Interventions in
*criticisms and interventions on behalf of child *dependence on institutions
Institutions
*training of child to take on this role
*sense of powerlessness and frustrations
*conflict between childrearing practices at
home and at school
Consequences
Emerging sense of entitlement on the part of
Emerging sense of constraint on the part of
the child
the child

Page 2
Lareau / Unequal Childhoods | 445
dle-class category based on discussions with each of
the employed adults. They provided extensive infor-
mation about the work they did, the nature of the
organization that employed them, and their educa-
tional credentials. I added a third category: families
not involved in the labor market (a population tradi-
tionally excluded from social class groupings) be-
cause in the first school I studied, a substantial
number of children were from households supported
by public assistance. to ignore them would have re-
stricted the scope of the study arbitrarily. The final
subsample contained four middle-class, four work-
ing-class, and four poor families.
Children’s Time Use
In our interviews and observations of white and
black middle-class children, it was striking how busy
they were with organized activities. Indeed, one of
the hallmarks of middle-class children’s daily lives is
a set of adult-run organized activities. Many children
have three and four activities per week. In some fam-
ilies, every few days, activities conflict, particularly
when one season is ending and one is beginning. For
example in the white middle-class family of the tall-
ingers, Garrett is on multiple soccer teams—the “a”
traveling team of the private Forest soccer club and
the Intercounty soccer team—he also has swim les-
sons, saxophone lessons at school, private piano les-
sons at home, and baseball and basketball. These
organized activities provided a framework for chil-
dren’s lives; other activities were sandwiched between
them.
These activities create labor for parents. Indeed,
the impact of children’s activities takes its toll on par-
ents’ patience as well as their time. For example, on a
June afternoon at the beginning of summer vacation,
in a white-middle-class family, Mr. tallinger comes
home from work to take Garrett to his soccer game.
Garrett is not ready to go, and his lackadaisical ap-
proach to getting ready irks his father:
Don says, “Get your soccer stuff—you’re
going to a soccer game!” Garrett comes into
the den with white short leggings on under-
neath a long green soccer shirt; he’s number
16. He sits on an armchair catty-corner from
the television and languidly watches the
World Cup game. He slowly, abstractedly,
pulls on shin guards, then long socks. His
eyes are riveted to the tV screen. Don comes
in: “Go get your other stuff.” Garrett says he
can’t find his shorts. Don: “Did you look in
your drawer?” Garrett nods. . . . He gets up
to look for his shorts, comes back into the
den a few minutes later. I ask, “any luck yet?”
Garrett shakes his head. Don is rustling
around elsewhere in the house. Don comes
in, says to Garrett, “Well, Garrett, aren’t you
wearing shoes?” (Don leaves and returns a
short time later): “Garrett, we HaVe to go!
Move! We’re late!” He says this shortly,
abruptly. He comes back in a minute and
drops Garrett’s shiny green shorts on his lap
without a word.
This pressured search for a pair of shiny green
soccer shorts is a typical event in the tallinger house-
hold. also typical is the solution—a parent ulti-
mately finds the missing object, while continuing to
prod the child to hurry. The fact that today’s frenzied
schedule will be matched or exceeded by the next
day’s is also par:
Don: (describing their day on Saturday) to-
morrow is really nuts. We have a soccer game,
then a baseball game, then another soccer
game.
This steady schedule of activity—that none of the
middle-class parents reported having when they were
a similar age—was not universal. Indeed, while we
searched for a middle-class child who did not have a
single organized activity, we could not find one, but
in working-class and poor homes, organized activi-
ties were much less common and there were many
children who did not have any. Many children
“hung out.” television and video games are a major
source of entertainment but outdoor play can trump
either of these. no advanced planning, no telephone
calls, no consultations between mothers, no drop-
offs or pickups—no particular effort at all—is re-
quired to launch an activity. For instance, one
afternoon, in a black working-class family, Shannon
(in 7th grade) and tyrec (in 4th grade) walk out
their front door to the curb of the small, narrow
street their house faces. Shannon begins playing a
game with a ball; she soon has company:
(two boys from the neighborhood walk up.)
Shannon is throwing the small ball against the
side of the row house. tyrec joins in the game
with her. as they throw the ball against the
wall, they say things they must do with the

Page 3
446 | PaRt VIII: HoW InequaLIty SPILLS oVeR
ball. It went something like this: Johnny Crow
wanted to know. . . . (bounces ball against the
wall), touch your knee (bounce), touch your
toe (bounce), touch the ground (bounce),
under the knee (bounce), turn around
(bounce). Shannon and tyrec played about
four rounds.
unexpected events produce hilarity:
at one point Shannon accidentally threw
the ball and it bounced off of tyrec’s head. all
the kids laughed; then tyrec, who had the
ball, went chasing after Shannon. It was a
close, fun moment—lots of laughter, eye con-
tact, giggling, chasing.
Soon a different game evolves. tyrec is on restric-
tion. He is supposed to remain inside the house all
day. So, when he thinks he has caught a glimpse of
his mom returning home from work, he dashes in-
side. He reappears as soon as he realizes that it was a
false alarm. The neighborhood children begin an in-
formal game of baiting him:
The kids keep teasing tyrec that his mom’s
coming—which sends him scurrying just in-
side the door, peering out of the screen door.
This game is enacted about six times. tyrec
also chases Shannon around the street, trying
to get the ball from her. a few times Shannon
tells tyrec that he’d better “get inside”; he ig-
nores her. Then, at 6:50 [P.M.] Ken (a friend
of tyrec’s) says, “There’s your mom!” tyrec
scoots inside, then says, “oh, man. you were
serious this time.”
Informal, impromptu outdoor play is common in
tyrec’s neighborhood. a group of boys approxi-
mately his age, regularly numbering four or five but
sometimes reaching as many as ten, play ball games
together on the street, walk to the store to get treats,
watch television at each other’s homes, and generally
hang out together.
Language Use
In addition to differences by social class in time use,
we also observed differences in language use in the
home. as others have noted (Bernstein, 1971; Heath,
1983) middle-class parents used more reasoning in
their speech with children while working-class and
poor parents used more directives. For example, in
observations of the african american home of alex
Williams, whose father was a trial lawyer and mother
was a high-level corporate executive, we found that
the Williamses and other middle-class parents use lan-
guage frequently, pleasurably, and instrumentally.
Their children do likewise. For example, one January
evening, alexander is stumped by a homework assign-
ment to write five riddles. He sits at the dinner table
in the kitchen with his mother and a fieldworker. Mr.
Williams is at the sink, washing the dinner dishes. He
has his back to the group at the dinner table. Without
turning around, he says to alex, “Why don’t you go
upstairs to the third floor and get one of those books
and see if there is a riddle in there?”
alex [says] smiling, “yeah. That’s a good idea!
I’ll go upstairs and copy one from out of the
book.” terry turns around with a dish in
hand, “That was a joke—not a valid sugges-
tion. That is not an option.” He smiled as he
turned back around to the sink. Christina
says, looking at alex: “There is a word for
that you know, plagiarism.” terry says (not
turning around), “Someone can sue you for
plagiarizing. Did you know that?” alex:
“That’s only if it is copyrighted.” They all
begin talking at once.
Here we see alex cheerfully (though gently)
goading his father by pretending to misunderstand
the verbal instruction to consult a book for help. Mr.
Williams dutifully rises to the bait. Ms. Williams re-
shapes this moment of lightheartedness by introduc-
ing a new word into alexander’s vocabulary. Mr.
Williams goes one step further by connecting the
new word to a legal consequence. alex upstages
them both. He demonstrates that he is already famil-
iar with the general idea of plagiarism and that he
understands the concept of copyright, as well.
In marked contrast to working-class and poor
parents, however, even when the Williamses issue di-
rectives, they often include explanations for their
orders. Here, Ms. Williams is reminding her son to
pay attention to his teacher:
I want you to pay close attention to Mrs. Scott
when you are developing your film. Those
chemicals are very dangerous. Don’t play
around in the classroom. you could get that
stuff in someone’s eye. and if you swallow it,
you could die.
alex chooses to ignore the directive in
favor of instructing his misinformed mother:

Page 4
Lareau / Unequal Childhoods | 447
alex corrects her, “Mrs. Scott told us that
we wouldn’t die if we swallowed it. But we
would get very sick and would have to get our
stomach pumped.” Christina does not follow
the argument any further. She simply reiter-
ates that he should be careful.
Possibly because the issue is safety, Ms. Williams
does not encourage alex to elaborate here, as she
would be likely to do if the topic were less-charged.
Instead, she restates her directive and thus under-
scores her expectation that alex will do as she asks.
although Mr. and Ms. Williams disagreed on ele-
ments of how training in race relations should be im-
plemented, they both recognized that their racial and
ethnic identity profoundly shaped their and their
son’s everyday experiences. They were well aware of
the potential for alexander to be exposed to racial
injustice, and they went to great lengths to try to pro-
tect their son from racial insults and other forms of
discrimination. nevertheless, race did not appear to
shape the dominant cultural logic of child rearing in
alexander’s family or in other families in the study.
all of the middle-class families engaged in extensive
reasoning with their children, asking questions, prob-
ing assertions, and listening to answers. Similar pat-
terns appeared in interviews and observations with
other african american middle-class families.
a different pattern appeared in working-class and
poor homes where there was simply less verbal
speech than we observed in middle-class homes.
There was also less speech between parents and chil-
dren, a finding noted by other observational studies
(Hart and Risley, 1995). Moreover, interspersed with
intermittent talk are adult-issued directives. Chil-
dren are told to do certain things (e.g., shower, take
out the garbage) and not to do others (e.g., curse,
talk back). In an african american home of a family
living on public assistance in public housing, Ms.
Mcallister uses one-word directives to coordinate
the use of the single bathroom. There are almost al-
ways at least four children in the apartment and
often seven, plus Ms. Mcallister and other adults.
Ms. Mcallister sends the children to wash up by
pointing to a child, saying, “Bathroom,” and hand-
ing him or her a washcloth. Wordlessly, the desig-
nated child gets up and goes to the bathroom to take
a shower.
Children usually do what adults ask of them. We
did not observe whining or protests, even when
adults assign time-consuming tasks, such as the
hour-long process of hair-braiding Lori Mcallister is
told to do for the four-year-old daughter of aunt
Dara’s friend Charmaine:
Someone tells Lori, “Go do [tyneshia’s] hair
for camp.” Without saying anything, Lori
gets up and goes inside and takes the little
girl with her. They head for the couch near
the television; Lori sits on the couch and the
girl sits on the floor. [tyneshia] sits quietly
for about an hour, with her head tilted, while
Lori carefully does a multitude of braids.
Lori’s silent obedience is typical. Generally, chil-
dren perform requests without comment. For exam-
ple, at dinner one night, after Harold Mcallister
complains he doesn’t like spinach, his mother directs
him to finish it anyway:
Mom yells (loudly) at him to eat: “eat!
FInISH tHe SPInaCH!” (no response.
Harold is at the table, dawdling.) Guion and
Runako and alexis finish eating and leave. I
finish with Harold; he eats his spinach. He
leaves all his yams.
The verbal world of Harold Mcallister and other
poor and working-class children offers some import-
ant advantages as well as costs. Compared to mid-
dle-class children we observed, Harold is more
respectful towards adults in his family. In this setting,
there are clear boundaries between adults and chil-
dren. adults feel comfortable issuing directives to
children, which children comply with immediately.
Some of the directives that adults issue center on ob-
ligations of children to others in the family (“don’t
beat on Guion” or “go do [her] hair for camp”). one
consequence of this is that Harold, despite occasional
tiffs, is much nicer to his sister (and his cousins) than
the siblings we observed in middle-class homes. The
use of directives and the pattern of silent compliance
are not universal in Harold’s life. In his interactions
with peers, for example on the basketball “court,”
Harold’s verbal displays are distinctively different
than inside the household, with elaborated and em-
bellished discourse. nevertheless, there is a striking
difference in linguistic interaction between adults
and children in poor and working-class families when
compared to that observed in the home of alexander
Williams. Ms. Mcallister has the benefit of being
able to issue directives without having to justify their
decisions at every moment. This can make childrear-
ing somewhat less tiring.

Page 5
448 | PaRt VIII: HoW InequaLIty SPILLS oVeR
another advantage is that Harold has more au-
tonomy than middle-class children in making im-
portant decisions in daily life. as a child, he controls
his leisure schedule. His basketball games are im-
promptu and allow him to develop important skills
and talents. He is resourceful. He appears less ex-
hausted than ten-year-old alexander. In addition, he
has important social competencies, including his
deftness in negotiating the “code of the street.”1 His
mother has stressed these skills in her upbringing, as
she impresses upon her children the importance of
“not paying no mind” to others, including drunks
and drug dealers who hang out in the neighborhoods
which Harold and alexis negotiate.
Still, in the world of schools, health care facilities,
and other institutional settings, these valuable skills
do not translate into the same advantages as the rea-
soning skills emphasized in the home of alexander
Williams and other middle-class children. Compared
to alexander Williams, Harold does not gain the de-
velopment of a large vocabulary, an increase of his
knowledge of science and politics, a set of tools to
customize situations outside the home to maximize
his advantage, and instruction in how to defend his
argument with evidence. His knowledge of words,
which might appear, for example, on future Sat
tests, is not continually stressed at home.
In these areas, the lack of advantage is not con-
nected to the intrinsic value of the Mcallister family
life or the use of directives at home. Indeed, one can
argue raising children who are polite and respectful
children and do not whine, needle, or badger their
parents is a highly laudable childrearing goal. Deep
and abiding ties with kinship groups are also, one
might further argue, important. Rather, it is the spe-
cific ways that institutions function that end up con-
veying advantages to middle-class children. In their
standards, these institutions also permit, and even
demand, active parent involvement. In this way as
well, middle-class children often gain an advantage.
Intervention in Institutions
Children do not live their lives inside of the home.
Instead, they are legally required to go to school,
they go to the doctor, and many are involved in
church and other adult-organized activities. In chil-
dren’s institutional lives, we found differences by so-
cial class in how mothers monitored children’s
institutional experiences. While in working-class and
poor families, children are granted autonomy to
make their own way in organizations, in the mid-
dle-class homes, most aspects of the children’s lives
are subject to their mother’s ongoing scrutiny.
For example, in an african american middle-class
home, where both parents are college graduates and
Ms. Marshall is a computer worker and her husband
a civil servant, their two daughters have a hectic
schedule of organized activities including gymnastics
for Stacey and basketball for Fern. When Ms. Mar-
shall becomes aware of a problem, she moves quickly,
drawing on her work and professional skills and ex-
periences. She displays tremendous assertiveness,
doggedness, and, in some cases, effectiveness in
pressing institutions to recognize her daughters’ in-
dividualized needs. Stacey’s mother’s proactive stance
reflects her belief that she has a duty to intervene in
situations where she perceives that her daughter’s
needs are not being met. This perceived responsibil-
ity applies across all areas of her children’s lives. She
is no more (or less) diligent with regard to Stacey
and Fern’s leisure activities than she is with regard to
their experiences in school or church or the doctor’s
office. This is clear in the way she handles Stacey’s
transition from her township gymnastics classes to
the private classes at an elite private gymnastic pro-
gram at Wright’s:
Ms. Marshall describes Stacey’s first session at the
club as rocky:
The girls were not warm. and these were lit-
tle . . . eight and nine year old kids. you
know, they weren’t welcoming her the first
night. It was kinda like eyeing each other, to
see, you know, “Can you do this? Can you do
that?”
More importantly, Ms. Marshall reported that the
instructor is brusque, critical, and not friendly to-
ward Stacey. Ms. Marshall cannot hear what was
being said, but she could see the interactions through
a window. a key problem is that because her previous
instructor had not used the professional jargon for
gymnastic moves, Stacey does not know these terms.
When the class ends and she walks out, she is visibly
upset. Her mother’s reaction is a common one among
middle-class parents: She does not remind her daugh-
ter that in life one has to adjust, that she will need to
work even harder, or that there is nothing to be done.
Instead, Ms. Marshall focuses on tina, the instructor,
as the source of the problem:
We sat in the car for a minute and I said,
“Look, Stac,” I said. She said, “I-I,” and she

Page 6
Lareau / Unequal Childhoods | 449
started crying. I said, “you wait here.” The
instructor had come to the door, tina. So I
went to her and I said, “Look.” I said, “Is
there a problem?” She said, “aww . . . she’ll
be fine. She just needs to work on certain
things.” Blah-blah-blah. and I said, “She’s re-
ally upset. She said you-you-you [were]
pretty much correcting just about every-
thing.” and [tina] said, “Well, she’s got—
she’s gotta learn the terminology.”
Ms. Marshall acknowledges that Stacey isn’t fa-
miliar with specialized and technical gymnastics
terms. nonetheless, she continues to defend her
daughter:
I do remember, I said to her, I said, “Look,
maybe it’s not all the student.” you know, I
just left it like that. That, you know, some-
times teaching, learning and teaching, is a
two-way proposition as far as I’m concerned.
and sometimes teachers have to learn how
to, you know, meet the needs of the kid. Her
style, her immediate style was not accommo-
dating to—to Stacey.
Here Ms. Marshall is asserting the legitimacy of an
individualized approach to instruction. She frames
her opening remark as a question (“Is there a prob-
lem?”). Her purpose, however, is to alert the instruc-
tor to the negative impact she has had on Stacey
(“She’s really upset.”). although her criticism is indi-
rect (“Maybe it’s not all the student . . . ”), Ms. Mar-
shall makes it clear that she expects her daughter to be
treated differently in the future. In this case, Stacey
does not hear what her mother says, but she knows
that her wishes and feelings are being transmitted to
the instructor in a way that she could not do herself.
although parents were equally concerned about
their children’s happiness, in working-class and
poor homes we observed different patterns of over-
sight for children’s institutional activities. For ex-
ample, in the white working-class home of Wendy
Driver, Wendy’s mother does not nurture her
daughter’s language development like alexander
Williams’ mother does her son’s. She does not at-
tempt to draw Wendy out or follow up on new in-
formation when Wendy introduces the term mortal
sin while the family is sitting around watching tele-
vision. But, just like Ms. Williams, Ms. Driver
cares very much about her child and just like mid-
dle-class parents she wants to help her daughter
succeed. Ms. Driver keeps a close and careful eye
on her Wendy’s schooling. She knows that Wendy
is having problems in school. Ms. Driver immedi-
ately signs and returns each form Wendy brings
home from school and reminds her to turn the pa-
pers in to her teacher.
Wendy is “being tested” as part of an ongoing ef-
fort to determine why she has difficulties with spell-
ing, reading, and related language-based activities.
Her mother welcomes these official efforts but she
did not request them. unlike the middle-class moth-
ers we observed, who asked teachers for detailed in-
formation about every aspect of their children’s
classroom performance and relentlessly pursued in-
formation and assessments outside of school as well,
Ms. Driver seems content with only a vague notion
of her daughter’s learning disabilities. This attitude
contrasts starkly with that of Stacey Marshall’s
mother, for example. In discussing Stacey’s classroom
experiences with fieldworkers, Ms. Marshall rou-
tinely described her daughter’s academic strengths
and weaknesses in detail. Ms. Driver never mentions
that Wendy is doing grade-level work in math but is
reading at a level a full three years below her grade.
Her description is vague:
She’s having problems. . . . They had a special
teacher come in and see if they could find out
what the problem is. She has a reading prob-
lem, but they haven’t put their finger on it
yet, so she’s been through all kinds of special
teachers and testing and everything. She goes
to Special ed, I think it’s two classes a day . . .
I’m not one hundred percent sure—for her
reading. It’s very difficult for her to read
what’s on paper. But then—she can remem-
ber things. But not everything. It’s like she
has a puzzle up there. and we’ve tried, well,
they’ve tried a lot of things. They just haven’t
put their finger on it yet.
Wendy’s teachers uniformly praise her mother as
“supportive” and describe her as “very loving,” but
they are disappointed in Ms. Driver’s failure to take
a more active, interventionist role in Wendy’s educa-
tion, especially given the formidable nature of her
daughter’s learning problems. From Ms. Driver’s
perspective, however, being actively supportive
means doing whatever the teachers tell her to do.
Whatever they would suggest, I would do.
They suggested she go to the eye doctor, so I

Page 7
450 | PaRt VIII: HoW InequaLIty SPILLS oVeR
did that. and they checked her and said there
was nothing wrong there.
Similarly, she monitors Wendy’s homework and
supports her efforts to read:
We listen to her read. We help her with her
homework. So she has more attention here in
a smaller household than it was when I lived
with my parents. So, we’re trying to help her
out more, which I think is helping. and with
the two [special education] classes a day at the
school, instead of one like last year, she’s
learning a lot from that. So, we’re just hoping
it takes time and that she’ll just snap out of it.
But Ms. Driver clearly does not have an indepen-
dent understanding of the nature or degree of Wen-
dy’s limitations, perhaps because she is unfamiliar
with the kind of terms the educators use to describe
her daughter’s needs (e.g., a limited “sight vocabu-
lary,” underdeveloped “language arts skills”). Per-
haps, too, her confidence in the school staff makes it
easier for her to leave “the details” to them: “Ms.
Morton, she’s great. She’s worked with us for differ-
ent testing and stuff.” Ms. Driver depends on the
school staff’s expertise to assess the situation and
then share the information with her:
I think they just want to keep it in the school
till now. and when they get to a point where
they can’t figure out what it is, and then I
guess they’ll send me somewhere else. . . .
Her mother is not alarmed, because “the school”
has told her not to worry about Wendy’s grades:
Her report card—as long as it’s not spelling
and reading—spelling and reading are like
F’s. and they keep telling me not to worry,
because she’s in the Special ed class. But be-
sides that, she does good. I have no behavior
problems with her at all.
Ms. Driver wants the best possible outcome for her
daughter and she does not know how to achieve that
goal without relying heavily on Wendy’s teachers:
I wouldn’t even know where to start going. on
the radio there was something for children
having problems reading and this and that,
call. and I suggested it to a couple different
people, and they were like, wait a second, it’s
only to get you there and you’ll end up paying
an arm and a leg. So I said to my mom, “no,
I’m going to wait until the first report card
and go up and talk to them up there.”
Thus, in looking for the source of Ms. Driver’s def-
erence toward educators, the answers don’t seem to lie
in her having either a shy personality or underdevel-
oped mothering skills. to understand why Wendy’s
mother is accepting where Stacey Marshall’s mother
would be aggressive, it is more useful to focus on so-
cial class position, both in terms of how class shapes
worldviews and how class affects economic and edu-
cational resources. Ms. Driver understands her role in
her daughter’s education as involving a different set of
responsibilities from those perceived by middle-class
mothers. She responds to contacts from the school—
such as invitations to the two annual parent-teacher
conferences—but she does not initiate them. She
views Wendy’s school life as a separate realm, and one
in which she, as a parent, is only an infrequent visitor.
Ms. Driver expects that the teachers will teach and her
daughter will learn and that, under normal circum-
stances, neither requires any additional help from her
as a parent. If problems arise, she presumes that
Wendy will tell her; or, if the issue is serious, the
school will contact her. But what Ms. Driver fails to
understand, is that the educators expect her to take on
a pattern of “concerted cultivation” where she actively
monitors and intervenes in her child’s schooling. The
teachers asked for a complicated mixture of deference
and engagement from parents; they were disappointed
when they did not get it.
Conclusions
I have stressed how social class dynamics are woven
into the texture and rhythm of children and parents’
daily lives. Class position influences critical aspects of
family life: time use, language use, and kin ties. Work-
ing-class and middle-class mothers may express beliefs
that reflect a similar notion of “intensive mothering,”
but their behavior is quite different. For that reason, I
have described sets of paired beliefs and actions as a
“cultural logic” of childrearing. When children and
parents move outside the home into the world of so-
cial institutions, they find that these cultural practices
are not given equal value. There are signs that mid-
dle-class children benefit, in ways that are invisible to

Page 8
Lutfey and Freese / The Fundamentals of Fundamental Causality | 451
them and to their parents, from the degree of similar-
ity between the cultural repertoires in the home and
those standards adopted by institutions.
REFERENCES
anderson, elijah. 1999. Code of the Street. new york, ny:
W. W. norton.
Bernstein, Basil. 1971. Class, Codes, and Control: Theoret-
ical Studies Towards a Sociology of Language. new york,
ny: Schocken.
Hart, Betty and todd R. Risley. 1995. Meaningful Differ-
ences in the Everyday Experiences of Young American Chil-
dren. new Haven: yale university Press.
Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with Words: Language,
Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge university Press.
Lareau, annette. 2003. Unequal Childhoods: Class, Race, and
Family Life. Berkeley, Ca: university of California Press.
NOTES
1. elijah anderson, Code of the Street, new york: W. W.
norton (1999).
68. Karen Lutfey and
Jeremy Freese*
The Fundamentals of
Fundamental Causality
Weber’s ([1921] 1968) concept of “life chances”
highlights both the diverse consequences of social
standing and their probabilistic character. The most
poignant kind of life chance affected by socioeco-
nomic standing may also be the most literal: the
probability of staying alive or dying. Lower socioeco-
nomic status (SeS) is associated with worse health
and higher mortality rates at virtually every age
*The ideas, issues, and theories considered in this brief commis-
sioned piece are examined in greater depth in the following
publication: Karen Lutfey and Jeremy Freese, “toward Some
Fundamentals of Fundamental Causality: Socioeconomic Status
and Health in the Routine Clinic Visit for Diabetes,” American
Journal of Sociology 110:5 (March 2005), pp. 1326–1372, pub-
lished by the university of Chicago Press. The article printed
here was originally prepared by Karen Lutfey and Jeremy Freese
for the fourth edition of Social Stratification: Class, Race, and
Gender in Sociological Perspective, edited by David B. Grusky.
Copyright � 2014 by Westview Press.
(Robert and House 1994), and this association has
persisted across historical periods in which risk fac-
tors and disease profiles have changed radically (Link
et al. 1998). We use ethnographic data to examine
the association between SeS and adverse health out-
comes among persons with diabetes.
We draw specifically on Link and Phelan’s (1995;
Phelan, Link, and tehranifar 2010) concept of SeS
as a “fundamental cause” of health. Fundamental
causality is not just about what causes an outcome
like ill-health, but about what causes the causes of ill-
health. That is, the concept is not about the specific
proximate mechanisms responsible for a persistent as-
sociation, but rather about the fact that some meta-
mechanism(s) are responsible for the generation of
multiple concrete mechanisms that reproduce a par-
ticular relationship in different places and times. The
metamechanism provides what we refer to as a dura-
ble narrative (Freese and Lutfey 2011) about why the
SeS-health relationship should be robust to changes
in health threats and treatments—an explanation of
why a similar association would be observed in di-
verse sociohistorical contexts.
a fundamental relationship between two vari-
ables such as SeS and health implies the potential
for a massive multiplicity of mechanisms connecting
the two. no individual mechanism is so dominant
that it alone is responsible for the bulk of the ob-
served association between SeS and health. Rather,
as proximate causes of health change, the standing
conjecture is that these will, on balance, sustain the
overall relationship between SeS and health. Differ-
ential resources provide a possible metamechanism
(Link and Phelan 1995). The ways resources can in-
fluence health are flexible and varied, allowing the
possibility that, when one uses ethnographic meth-
ods to consider in a concrete setting how different
resources might lead to different outcomes, many
potential mechanisms will be revealed.
Diabetes
Diabetes is a major cause of morbidity and mortal-
ity in the united States, and its prevalence is in-
creasing dramatically (Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention 2011). Diabetes incidence, compli-
cations (Booth and Hux 2003; Robbins et al.
2001), and mortality have all been shown to be
related to SeS. Because diabetes complications are
known to be linked to average glucose levels (Diabe-
tes Control and Complications trial Research Group