This is the html version of the file https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Adejoke-Onaolapo/publication/323781878_Global_Data_on_Autism_Spectrum_Disorders_Prevalence_A_Review_of_Facts_Fallacies_and_Limitations/links/5aadc4d2458515ecebe7d515/Global-Data-on-Autism-Spectrum-Disorders-Prevalence-A-Review-of-Facts-Fallacies-and-Limitations.pdf.
Google automatically generates html versions of documents as we crawl the web.
These search terms have been highlighted: diagnosis autism male preponderance
Page 1
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
http://www.hrpub.org
DOI: 10.13189/ujcm.2017.050202
Global Data on Autism Spectrum Disorders Prevalence:
A Review of Facts, Fallacies and Limitations
Onaolapo A Y1, Onaolapo O J2,*
1Behavioural Neuroscience/Neurobiology Unit, Department of Anatomy, Ladoke Akintola University of Technology,
Ogbomoso, Oyo State, Nigeria
2Behavioural Neuroscience/Neuropharmacology Unit, Department of Pharmacology, Ladoke Akintola University of Technology,
Osogbo, Osun State, Nigeria
Copyright�2017 by authors, all rights reserved. Authors agree that this article remains permanently open access under
the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 International License
Abstract Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a
range of neurodevelopmental disorders whose aetiologies
are largely unknown. In the past few decades, studies have
demonstrated that ASDs occur globally, and that the
numbers of recorded cases are rising; however,
determining the true prevalence figures is still a major
challenge, especially in developing nations. Also, subtle
differences in cultural norms might impede timely/accurate
diagnosis and categorisation of patients. In this review, we
examine the globally-available data on the prevalence of
ASD and discuss some of the challenges of data acquisition,
with reference to how these may impact the reliability of
figures obtained. Some of the facts, fallacies and
limitations relating to the presently-available figures are
also highlighted.
Keywords Neurodevelopment, Aetiology, Prevalence,
Autism, Data
1. Introduction
Epidemiology is not only concerned with patterns of
disease occurrence within human populations, but it is also
concerned with the factors that determine or influence
development of disease, or maintenance of health. Over the
years, different study designs (surveillance, descriptive,
analytical, cohort, case-control and cross-sectional) have
been employed in examining the relationships that may
exist between disease and disease determinants in defined
human populations. Generally, epidemiological
investigations of neuropsychiatric diseases begin with
cross-sectional surveys, which help to determine the
prevalence of the disease condition, patterns of risk factors,
as well as the possible correlates between disease and risk
factors among representative samples [1]. The first
epidemiological survey of autism was initiated in England
in the early to mid-1960s [2, 3], while the first
hospital-based survey in sub-Saharan Africa was
conducted in five countries in the late 1970s [4]. Autism
surveys have since become global, with different countries
conducting surveys to determine the prevalence and risk
factors in their communities [1, 5], and thereby propose
strategies to improve identification, diagnosis,
management as well as promote policy reforms that bring
awareness to the plight of the autistic child.
The term ‘Autism’ derives from the Greek word "autos",
meaning "self”; and it was in the year 1911 that Eugen
Bleuler first used the word autism in describing a symptom
of schizophrenia. However, in 1943 and 1944 respectively,
Leo Kanner (USA) and Hans Asperger (Germany)
described individuals with social/emotional limitations and
withdrawn behaviours. Kanner reported behavioural
aberrations such as poor social interaction, obsessiveness,
stereotypic movement, and echolalia in the affected
children. It was Kanner who first coined the term ‘infantile
autism’ (earlier called Kanner’s syndrome) to describe his
observations in this group of children who seemed
socially-isolated and withdrawn [6]; while Asperger named
the condition he observed Asperger’s syndrome. Decades
of research will eventually reveal that both syndromes are
parts of a highly-variable spectrum of disorders. Autism
spectrum disorders (ASDs) are an array of
neurodevelopmental disorders characterised by cognitive
and neurobehavioural deficits, whose underlying factors
are genetic and/or neurobiological [7]. In the Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual (DSM) III, autism debuted in 1980
as "infantile autism", which was replaced by the term
"Autistic Disorder" in 1987. In the DSM IV, it was referred
to as pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs); and
consisted of autistic disorder (autism), Asperger disorder,
childhood disintegrative disorder, Rett syndrome, and
PDD-not otherwise specified (NOS).
Over the years, a lot has changed about our perception of
ASDs, the perceived risk factors, core diagnostic criteria,

Page 2
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
15
and even approaches to management. Presently, according
to the DSM V, persistent deficits in social communication/
social interaction and a restricted/repetitive (stereotypical)
patterns of behaviour, interests or activities; which must be
present in the early developmental period(but might not
become obvious until social demands exceed limited
capacity), cause clinically significant impairment (in social,
occupational or other important areas of current
functioning) and must not be better explained by
intellectual disability or global developmental delay, are
the core criteria for the diagnosis of ASD [8]. DSM 5 also
makes provision for differential diagnosis such as ‘social
communication disorder’. Level of severity of symptoms
{3 levels given for both social communication/social
interaction, and restricted/repetitive (stereotypical) patterns
of behaviour} determines the degree of supportive care
needed, with level 3 severity requiring very substantial
support. Apart from the core symptoms, patients often have
co-morbid psychiatric and behavioural manifestations like
self-injury, aggression, impulsivity, hyperactivity, anxiety
and mood symptoms; all of which may worsen the overall
prognosis, stress the caregivers, and further complicate
management. Recently, results from epidemiological
surveys indicate that the global prevalence of ASDs is on
the rise; although data from sub-Saharan Africa and a
number of other developing countries still show numbers
that are generally lower than those from the developed
countries [9-11]. Whether this reflects an absolute low
prevalence, deficits in diagnostic skills, mal-adaptation of
diagnostic criteria as it relates to cultural differences in
behaviour, or under-sampling is still being studied.
The aetiology of ASD is still being studied, and despite
years of research, a complete understanding of the
causative factors is still elusive; and while it is now known
that genetics may plays a big role in ASD, a rapidly
increasing prevalence suggests a bigger role of
environmental factors.
Generally, abnormalities in neural connectivity
involving synapses, tracts and neuronal communication via
neurotransmitters are key pathological features of the brain
in autism [12]. Neurodevelopmental defects in synapse
formation/elimination and changes in ratios of
inhibitory/excitatory synapses leads to defective local and
long range connectivity [12]. An active neuroinflammatory
process involving the cerebral cortex and the cerebellum;
with associated loss of cerebellar Purkinje cells, marked
reactivity of the Bergmann’s astroglia in areas of Purkinje
cell loss, marked astroglial reactions in the granule cell
layer and cerebellar white matter, and astroglial reactions
in the middle frontal/cingulate gyri had been demonstrated
by neuropathological examinations [13].
This
neuroinflammation may have a strong link to
autoimmunity, based on demonstrable elevated levels of
immunoglobulins (IgG, IgM and IgA) against select
neuron-specific antigens in ASD children [14]; and a
significantly higher presence of serum auto-antibodies to
the human brain (notably the cerebellum and cingulate
gyrus) in autistic children) [15].
The cause(s) of the above-mentioned pathological
changes are traceable to both genetics and the environment.
The heterogeneity of ASD is not only reflected in its
behavioural manifestations, but also in the genetic basis, as
many candidate ASD genes have been studied or
implicated [16]. Also, while studies may differ in terms of
their conclusions regarding the degree of involvement of
genetics, it is generally accepted that genetics plays a
crucial role in ASD. Presently, candidate genes that have
been implicated in ASD include those that encode for
neural adhesion molecules, ion channels, scaffold proteins,
protein kinases, receptor/carrier proteins, signaling
modulator molecules, and circadian relevant proteins [17].
Environmental factors that had been linked to
ASD-associated neuroinflammation and immune
dysfunction include early-life exposure to various
xenobiotics including heavy metals such as lead, mercury
and aluminium [18]. The Ethyl mercury compound
(Thimerosal) is a bacteriostatic agent that was a component
of a number of childhood vaccines in Western nations. The
possibility of its involvement in ASD led to its removal
from many vaccines meant for children; and while it must
be noted that this was not accompanied by a sharp decline
in ASD prevalence, its continued use in vaccines given to
pregnant women further complicates the picture [18]. The
use of aluminium(Al) as an adjuvant in many vaccines has
continued, due to the fact that Al salts help to stimulate the
immune system to yield adequate antibody titres [19].
However, there is abundant scientific literature on the
neurotoxic effects of Al; and its ability to adversely affect
both the immune and the nervous systems, hence making it
a plausible risk factor for triggering ASD [18]. Al’s ability
to alter neuronal gene expression can also affect the
response of neurons to environmental insults such as toxins
[18]. The presence and abundance of environmental risk
factors may be a major determinant of geographical
variations in the true prevalence figures of ASD; however,
the wide variety of candidate environmental risk factors
makes matching environmental factors to prevalence a
challenging task.
There have also been reports suggesting that some of the
behavioural characteristics of “autistic” children in
developing countries differ, when compared to those in
developed nations [20, 21]. Along this line, how
prevalence figures are influenced by the beliefs and
cultural heritages of the different regions of the world is a
matter of debate. In this review, we discuss the global
epidemiology of ASDs by highlighting the incontrovertible
facts relating to its prevalence, fallacies of
misinterpretation of available figures as they are, and
limitations of data acquisition, diagnosis and categorisation
of patients. We also summarise how these factors impact
strategies to improve identification, diagnosis and
management; as well as promote policy reforms.

Page 3
16
Global Data on Autism Spectrum Disorders Prevalence: A Review of Facts, Fallacies and Limitations
1.1. Global Prevalence of ASDs
1.1.1. Prevalence in USA, Europe, China, Japan, Middle
East, Ecuador, Brazil and Mexico
Global data on the prevalence of disorders that now fall
under the term ASDs had been available before 2013. In a
2012 review that compared prevalence data from different
parts of the world (excluding sub-Saharan Africa), the
researchers estimated the median global prevalence of
17/10,000 (approx. 1 in 588) for autistic disorder, and
62/10,000 (approx. 1 in 161) for all pervasive
developmental disorders; while suggesting that the
increase in estimates over time and the variability between
countries and regions may be linked to diagnostic
switching, the broadening diagnostic criteria, service
availability and the increasing global autism awareness
[22]. Even prior to this, the epidemiology of ASDs had
portrayed a shifting dynamics, with prevalence increasing
from 2-4/10,000 children as reported in the 1940s [23], to
approximately 4-5/10,000 children, according to the
studies that were published in the 1960s and 1970s [24, 25],
11/1000 children in the USA, according to a survey
conducted about a decade ago [26]; while a Centres for
Disease Control and Prevention surveillance report
revealed a combined estimated prevalence of 14.6 per
1,000, or 1 in 68 children (aged 8 years, whose parents or
guardians resided in 11 ADDM Network sites) in the
United States [27]. The most recent CDC figures on ASD
published in 2015 (which are believed to be more accurate)
say 1 in 45 children in the USA have ASD; this is however
based on a parent survey which was designed to track the
prevalence of developmental disorders in children aged 3
to 17 years [28]. Studies in Europe recorded before 1999
reported prevalence rates ranging from 1.9 to 72.6 per
10,000 with a median of 18.75/10,000; while studies
conducted after 1999 reported rates ranging from 30.0/10
000 to 116.1/10 000, with a median rate of 61.9/10 000 [22].
In the United Kingdom, a 2006 study in 9-10 year-olds
reported a prevalence of childhood autism as 38.9/10,000,
while other ASDs (DSM III) was 77.2/10,000 (52.1-102.3),
making the total prevalence of all ASDs 116.1/10,000 [29].
However, in 2014, the National Autistic Society reported
that 1 out of every 100 children has ASD [30]. In China, it
was estimated that 1.1 in every 1,000 children are
diagnosed with autism [31]; while in an analysis of five
studies from mainland China, an estimated pooled mean
prevalence of 24.4 per 10,000 children was observed [32].
In a 2012 review of epidemiological studies conducted in
the Western Pacific region (including Japan and China)
since 2000, Elsabbagh et al. [22] reported that prevalence
rates varied from 2.8/10 000 to 94/10 000 with a median
value of 11.6/10 000. In the Middle East, the prevalence of
ASD was reported to be 1.4/10,000 in Oman, 29/10,000 in
the United Arab Emirates, and 4.3/10,000 in Bahrain
[33-37].
In Canada, the National Epidemiological Database for
the Study of Autism in Canada (NEDSAC) ranks ASD as
one of the most common developmental disabilities,
affecting 1 in 94 children [38]. A pilot study that was
conducted in Brazil reported a prevalence rate of 27/10,000
[39]; while the prevalence of ASD in regular
schoolchildren in Quito, Ecuador was found to be
11/10,000 persons [40]. The Leon survey in Mexico
reported an overall prevalence of 87/10, 000; a rate that
was deemed consistent with other studies conducted at a
similar period [41].
1.1.2. Prevalence of Autism in Sub-Saharan Africa, India
and the Caribbean Islands
According to the World Health Organisation, the global
prevalence of ASD has been estimated as 1 per 160 persons
[42]. However, the contribution of sub-Saharan Africa
(SSA) to this burden is largely unknown due to a paucity of
population-based surveys on autism in this region [10, 11,
22, 42, 43]. Data (largely from hospital-based studies) are
however available from a few countries in the region. In
south-west Nigeria, Lagunju et al [44] reported a
prevalence rate of 2.3% (1 in 43.5) amongst 2,320
first-time attendees of a neurology or child psychiatry
clinic. In another clinic-based study in south-eastern
Nigeria, prevalence of ASD was reported to be 0.8% (1 in
125) of the total number of children attending the clinic for
that year [45]; while in Uganda, a survey of 1,169 children
aged between 2 and 9 years reported an unadjusted
prevalence for ASD of 6.8/1000 [46]. All studies reported a
male predilection, and middle to high socioeconomic status
improved the likelihood of a child being evaluated for ASD.
While these studies provided some data, the absence of
data from large scale epidemiological studies from a
number of countries in this region have been suggested as a
possible reason for the perceived low incidence and/or
prevalence of autism observed in SSA [10, 11].
Until recently, India (like SSA) had a paucity of
community-based population studies for autism; although
there was a number of hospital-based autism clinical
assessment studies [47-49]. However, in a 2017
population-based study (of 28, 078 children aged between
1 and 10 years, cutting across geographical regions that
represent rural, urban, and tribal populations in India) using
the Hindi version of the Indian Scale for Assessment of
Autism; a prevalence of 0.15% (1 in 667) was reported,
with a male sex predilection [50]. Also, a high
socioeconomic status increased the likelihood of an ASD
diagnosis [50]. The prevalence of ASD in the study was
observed to be higher amongst children aged between 4
and 10 years with majority of them diagnosed with mild
autism; while amongst children aged between 1 and 7 years,
the diagnosis of moderate autism was prevalent. This was
however attributed to families delaying presentations till
the onset of delayed motor and speech development [50].
In an earlier study, the same authors (sampling 11,000
children within the same age range, and from the same

Page 4
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
17
region) reported a prevalence of 0.0009 % (1 in 1100), with
low socioeconomic status correlating positively with
autism [51].
In the Caribbean islands, the Aruba Autism Project
determined the prevalence of ASDs in birth years
1990–1999 in Aruba, and found a figure of 53/10,000 [52].
2. Autism Spectrum Disorders: The
Facts and Fallacies
Autism fact sheets from different advocacy groups
(Autism Society, National Autism Association, National
Autism Network, Talk About Curing Autism, National
Autism Network, Autism South Africa, Action In Autism,
Autism Speaks etc.) irrespective of region or country
generally accept and reiterate that autism is a
bio-neurological developmental disability which generally
appears before the age of 3; impacting normal brain
development in the areas of social interaction,
communication skills, and cognitive function. There have
also been suggestions that autism is a disease of developed
countries, with a male sex predilection, that does not affect
life expectancy. While currently, there is yet no cure for
autism, early diagnosis and treatment helps to improve the
diverse symptoms associated with autism.
2.1. ASDs Occur Globally
In relation to the prevalence of ASDs, data emanating
from developed nations appear more comprehensive and
reliable, compared to those from developing nations.
Despite this, one fact that can no longer be denied is that as
it stands today, ASDs occur globally irrespective of culture,
geography or degree of industrialisation; with variable
degrees of tangible data from different regions of the world
where studies had been conducted. Hence, contrary to what
might have been believed in the past, especially in the
developing countries, it is not a disorder of ‘the West’ or of
advanced nations. The realisation of this fact is of
particular importance in SSA and other regions of the
world where awareness levels are still growing.
2.2. ASD Prevalence Figures are Still Rising
Globally, the recorded prevalence figures for ASDs are
rising; and from a statistical point of view, this is
undeniable when we examine data from both developed
and developing countries. However, in many developing
countries, the reported rates continue to be significantly
lower than the developed countries [9]. Whether this truly
reflects an absolute low prevalence, deficits in diagnostic
skills, mal-adaptation of diagnostic criteria as it relates to
cultural differences in behaviour, or under sampling are
issues that continue to be discussed. The idea that the low
prevalence rates observed in SSA could be due to
underreporting stemmed from studies conducted amongst
children born to African immigrants in Sweden [53-55],
that reported high prevalence rates in this subset of the
population, compared to the indigenous population. The
possibility of late diagnosis or misdiagnosis has also been
considered, and studies have shown that African children
are more likely (than Caucasian children) to have a late
diagnosis of ASD [56], or a misdiagnosis [57].
Despite the lower figures (when compared to developed
nations), it is safe to say that even in SSA, the prevalence
rates of ASDs are on the rise; and there has been an
‘improvement’ from an era of no prevalence figures [22] to
the availability of hospital-based [44], or
community–based [46] prevalence figures. Also, in India,
the prevalence from a 2017 study [38] revealed a rise from
an earlier 2015 study [51].
In developed nations, a number of researchers have
attributed the rise in ASD prevalence to an increasing
ability to diagnose, or a possible reflection of the success of
public health systems in better-identifying children who
were previously undiagnosed [58]; and this is also related
to the continued redefinition of diagnostic criteria [22]. An
increased awareness is also thought to be largely
responsible for changing figures in less developed regions
of the world, such as SSA [10, 11], and Asia [50, 51].
Comparing the two Indian studies cited earlier [50, 51], it is
obvious that more children were recruited in the 2017 study
than were in the 2015; suggesting that in the 2 year period,
more families were aware enough to want to seek diagnosis
and treatment for their wards. There is also evidence of
increasing awareness, cultural reorientation and the
availability of well-trained child and adolescent care
specialists and allied professionals in SSA. In the last
decade, particularly in the last 5 years, the number of ASD
related studies in this region [44, 46, 59-61] has increased
considerably, compared to the preceding decades.
A review of a recent CDC data published in the National
Health Statistics Reports [28] also supports the theory that
changes in awareness, earlier diagnosis, and redefinition of
diagnostic criteria has a lot of impact on prevalence figures.
While the autism rate appeared to rise 79 % over a 3-year
period, the rate of developmental delay dropped 36 % [28];
and the combined prevalence of children diagnosed with
autism, developmental delay and intellectual disability did
not change over the 3 years. The unchanging statistic of the
sum data suggests that a number of children who were once
tagged as having developmental delay or intellectual
disability are now being identified as having autism [28].
In a study involving Danish children, Hansen and
colleagues [62] were also of the opinion that the increase in
prevalence of ASD was actually due to changes in how the
disease is diagnosed [62]. By using data from a large study
involving 677, 915 Danish children (born between 1980
and 1991) and tracked until they either had an autism
diagnosis or reached the terminal date of the study (Dec. 31,
2011); the authors concluded that significantly more

Page 5
18
Global Data on Autism Spectrum Disorders Prevalence: A Review of Facts, Fallacies and Limitations
children were diagnosed with autism in 1995 and thereafter,
than before 1994 (when autism became a spectrum of
disorders in Denmark) [62]. However, the authors also
noted what appears to be a certain degree of real increase in
prevalence.
Several researchers also share the opinion that there is a
real global rise in prevalence of ASDs, and the rise may be
the product of several factors such as exposure to
environmental toxins and increasing parental age/genetic
susceptibility [63-65]. Immune dysregulation in pregnancy
has also been linked to the rising prevalence of ASD; and
autoimmune inflammatory disorders, such as coeliac
disease and rheumatoid arthritis in mothers are believed to
increase the risk [66]. Inflammation due to common
infections and endemic parasitic illnesses are probably not
likely associated with increased risk; and this is consistent
with the observation that in some developing nations such
as Nigeria or Cambodia, where parasitic and infectious
diseases are still common, recorded ASD prevalence is still
low [45, 67].
The continued increase in prevalence figures of ASDs in
developed countries led to the postulation of the “Biome
Depletion Theory” by some researchers, who believe that
maternal immune response overreaction is a factor
underlying the development of ASDs in offsprings.
According to this theory, our immune system has
co-evolved alongside microbes and parasites, but a lack of
these pathogens in urban, post-industrial societies leads to
immune system over reactivity [67]. Along this line, it was
suggested that pro-biotics may be able to control the
inflammatory response in pregnant women, and prevent
some cases of ASD; however, this notion still seems like a
shot in the dark, and studies will have to be conducted to
examine this.
The apparent induced immune system over-reactivity
also formed the basis of an implied link between
vaccination and ASDs. In 1998, a case series published by
Wakefield and colleagues, but later retracted by 10 of the
12 authors suggested that the measles, mumps, and rubella
(MMR) vaccine could predispose to behavioural symptoms
that are representative of ASDs in children. Despite certain
obvious shortcomings of this study {including the very
small sample size (n=12)}; the publication generated a
social firestorm that led to some parents rejecting
vaccination for their children. However, epidemiological
studies conducted thereafter failed to establish such a link
[68, 69]. The CDC also continues to reiterate its position
that no such links exist. Again, a number of developing
countries who have contributed to the global data on ASD
prevalence do not have immunisation regimens like MMR;
and even if they do, data establishing a link appear
non-existent. From the foregoing, the following are
apparent: a) an undeniable deduction from the study by
Hansen et al [62]is that is not all new cases of ASDs are
explicable by changes in diagnostic ability; hence, it might
be unscientific and over simplistic to attribute all to
improved ability to diagnosis b) The public response that
followed the Wakefield article attests to the societal and
social burden of ASDs, and the dangers of the inability of
science to find a sound and timely answer in relation to the
cause(s) of a disorder that continues to plague more
children. It is therefore imperative to continue to research
and identify environmental and social factors that may
increase risk of ASDs.
2.3. ASD has a Male Sex Predilection
Sexual predilections in disease prevalence are
well-recognised, and female predilections have been
reported consistently in a number of disorders with
autoimmune aetiologies [70] while a male preponderance
has been observed in some neurodevelopmental disorders
[71-73] ASDs have also been observed to have a higher
prevalence in males, compared to age-matched females; in
a 4:1 ratio. This observation has been consistent across
populations, regions and time; strongly suggesting the
involvement of sex-specific biological factors in ASD
aetiology [74, 75]. Genetic studies have demonstrated that
females are protected from the effects of de novo and
heritable ASD risk variants, with suggestions that sex
hormones or sex chromosomal genes may modulate these
genetic variations [74], for a detailed review on the
influence of sex in autism (see Halladay et al., [75]).
2.4. ASD and Life-expectancy
It is generally believed that autism per se does not affect
life expectancy; however, research continues to show
higher mortality risk among individuals with ASD. In
comparison with mortality statistics from the general
population or general population controls, the risk of
premature mortality has been estimated to be 2-fold to
10-fold higher in the ASD population; even autistic adults
without a learning disability are 9 times more likely than
controls, to die by suicide [76]. Also, risk of mortality is
higher in females, compared to males [77]. Accidents such
as drowning and co-morbid medical conditions such as
epilepsy are among the classic causes of death; however,
these cannot fully account for the life span gap between
autistic and non-autistic people, or the difference in
mortality between autistic people with and without an
intellectual disability [76]. One of the factors that could
account for this gap is an increase in the prevalence of
health problems such as diabetes and respiratory disease;
and the fact that these health problems may go unnoticed
until it is too late [76]. Among autistic adults, pressures of
meeting the obligations of ‘normal life’ may lead to
continued isolation, depression and suicide. The
relationship between ASD and life-expectancy deserves
further studies, especially, with the realisation that certain
life-threatening medical conditions are more common in
ASD patients. Therefore, it might be erroneous to simply

Page 6
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
19
state that ASD does not affect life-expectancy without
explaining the ever-increasing complexity of the
relationship.
3. ASD Global Epidemiology:
Limitations of Acquisition,
Interpretation and Viability of Data
An accurate global comparison of data relating to the
prevalence of ASDs is a rather challenging task, due to
factors such as; a) the absence of data from large scale
epidemiological studies from a number of countries,
especially in the sub-Saharan countries like Nigeria (where
most of the available rates emanated from hospital-based
studies); even, developed nations like the USA cannot
boast of a truly all-inclusive national survey, probably due
to impediments of cost effectiveness and logistics b) the
information gathering tools are not totally adaptable to or
may not yield 100 % accuracy in some regions of the world
because of certain cultural peculiarities that might affect
diagnosis or categorisation c) there could be wide regional
variations in figures arising from the same country, as is the
case with China [32].
3.1. Absence of Population-based Prevalence Data in
Developing Countries
The prevalence figures of ASD in sub-Saharan Africa
have been based mainly on a few hospital-based studies,
and hardly any community-based study, except that of
Kakooza-Mwesige et al [46] in Uganda. Under these
circumstances, under-sampling or ‘skewed’ sampling have
been suggested to negatively impact the reported
prevalence [10, 11]. The study by Lagunju et al [44] was
conducted in a tertiary hospital setting in south-west
Nigeria, while that of Bakare et al [45] was conducted in a
similar setting in south-eastern Nigeria. These centres are
the ultimate referral centres for paediatric patients in their
respective regions and they represent a ‘gathering point’
for all ‘puzzling’ and ‘unusual’ childhood illnesses. First, it
must be noted that there are other regions (in the country),
for which there are no data. Secondly, confidently drawing
an inference from such studies is associated with two
potential pitfalls; that of under-representation, due to an
apparent small sample size, compared to the total
population of children within the age range; or of
over-representation, because the sampling frame is
compelled to fall on an area where such cases are likely to
cluster. This clustering effect may the apparent from the
figures; in Nigeria, Lagunju et al [44] sampled from the
paediatric neurology clinic and got a prevalence of 23/1000
and Bakare et al [45] sampled from a general paediatric
clinic to return a figure of 8/1000; while in Uganda,
Kakooza-Mwesige et al [46] sampled from the community
and got an unadjusted figure of 6.8/1000. It is obvious that
the true figures are likely to be gotten from the community,
and the present figures should be treated with caution until
larger community-based studies are carried out. However,
while the value of an epidemiological study for assessment
of needs and priorities within a community cannot be over-
emphasised; the cost implication of such studies may be
high, with a number of developing countries not finding
them easily-affordable, especially in the face of pressures
from other competing priorities.
3.2. Impact of Culture
Cultural differences play a large role in disease diagnosis,
especially when considering the diagnosis of disorders that
tends to attract a lot of social stigma; therefore, despite
adequate sensitisation and awareness, parents may still
refuse to submit their children for ASD evaluation. For
instance, in South Korea, a risk of intense stigmatisation
makes many families of children with developmental
delays to intentionally avoid diagnosis of ASD; also
cultures that believe that speech development in the male
child is usually slower than females make seeking early
help for the child to be impossible [9]. Therefore, societal
or parental behaviours affect ASD prevalence figures.
The lack of a culturally-adaptable tool for ASD
screening constitutes another impediment to having
reliable prevalence data; since culture/language differences
might affect diagnosis rates. Different cultures have
peculiar behaviours that are at least acceptable, if not even
desirable. Diagnosis of ASD is not based on laboratory
tests, but rather, on the exhibition of certain behaviours.
These behaviours include the tendency to establish eye
contact, ability to form relationships with others,
milestones in language development, and the presence of
certain repetitive motor and play behaviours. Cultural
perceptions affect the exhibition of at least some of these
behaviours; and in different cultures, parental perceptions
of their presence or otherwise affect the timing of seeking
help for a child with possible ASD. Therefore, behaviours
that are considered normal in some cultures may fall within
the inclusion criteria for ASD diagnosis.
Generally, in Europe and America, children are expected
and encouraged to make eye contact with others; but in
other parts of the world such as Asia and Africa, or even
among native communities in America and Europe, this is
not encouraged, and may even be perceived as rudeness or
insubordination, especially, when interacting with adults.
This has made certain researchers to be of the opinion that
the matter of use of eye contact as a diagnostic criterion for
autism needs to be handled with caution. There are also
cultural variations in the degree of a child’s interaction
with others, especially with adults. There are cultures all
over the world (Africa, Asia, Central and South America)
that don’t encourage ‘undue’ interactions between children
and adults (especially strangers). How children in these
cultures form relationships may not be exactly what is

Page 7
20
Global Data on Autism Spectrum Disorders Prevalence: A Review of Facts, Fallacies and Limitations
taken to be normal in urban Europe or America; and ASD
diagnosis in these communities may face an uphill task
when the children encounter these ‘strangers’(healthcare
personnel) that are questioning, examining and observing
them. Along this line also, language development may be
difficult to assess with all certainty, as the child may simply
be scared or ‘ashamed’ to talk to an unfamiliar adult;
leading to a situation where the parents vouch that the child
could speak, but he/she will refuse to communicate during
the assessment.
Since cultural differences may impair diagnosis of ASDs,
it is imperative to develop universally-adaptable diagnostic
criteria that are applicable to all cultures. In order to
achieve this, studies must be conducted across cultures to
find common markers that consistently differentiate
children with ASD from typically-developing children.
These markers are to be inserted as probes when gathering
data from communities. This gives an opportunity to
conduct a thorough and truly-comparable epidemiological
research, which gives rise to more reliable figures. Until
this has been achieved, it might be erroneous to simply
assume that the same criteria can be applied across
different cultures, without the need for culture-based
adjustments.
4. Conclusions
While a lot of progress had been made in the global
awareness of ASD, much remains to be done in order to
have a more accurate picture of the trend or global burden
of the disorder; and as science strives to understand more
about it, our minds need to be continuously open to do
away with certain erroneous or inaccurate impressions.
However, reaching an accurate diagnosis, accessing
therapy, acquiring epidemiological data and determining
true prevalence figures are still major challenges,
especially in developing nations. Also, research needs to
focus on dealing with cultural and social
norms/peculiarities that may hinder early diagnosis and
application of appropriate interventions. Continuous
development and application of culture-friendly and yet
globally-comparable diagnostic tools will go a long way in
bridging the gap in the ability to diagnose ASD that exists
between developing and developed nations. Finally, large
scale epidemiological studies will help towards obtaining a
truer picture of the prevalence of ASD in developing
countries, especially in sub-Saharan Africa.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
This article does not contain any studies with human
participants or animals performed by any of the authors.
Conflict of Interest
Onaolapo AY declares that she has no conflict of interest.
Onaolapo OJ also declares that he has no conflict of
interest.
Acknowledgements
This research did not receive any specific grant from
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit
sectors.
REFERENCES
[1] Fombonne E (1999) The epidemiology of autism: A review.
Psychological Medicine. 29, 769-86
[2] Lotter V (1966) Epidemiology of autistic conditions in
young children: I. Prevalence. Social Psychiatry 1, 124–137.
[3] Lotter V (1967) Epidemiology of autistic conditions in
young children: II. Some characteristics of the parents and
children. Social Psychiatry 1, 163–173.
[4] Lotter V (1978) Childhood autism in Africa. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 19(3), 231 – 244.
[5] Hill AP, Zuckerman K., Fombonne E (2015) Chapter 2
Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. In M. d. l. A.
Robinson-Agramonte (ed.) Translational Approaches to
Autism Spectrum Disorder, DOI
10.1007/978-3-319-16321-5_2
[6] Kanner L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact.
Nervous Child 2, 217-50.
[7] Kim SK. (2015) Recent update of autism spectrum disorders
Korean Journal of Pediatrics. 58, 8-14
[8] APA, American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed.
Washington, DC. pp. 50–59.
[9] Maguire C (2013) Autism on the Rise: A Global Perspective.
Harvard College, Global health Review. 4 (1)
[10] Abubakar A, Ssewanyana D, Newton CR (2016) A
Systematic review of research on autism spectrum disorders
in sub-Saharan Africa. Behavioural Neurology. 3501910,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/3501910
[11] Franz L, Chambers N, von Isenburg M, de Vries PJ (2017)
Autism spectrum disorder in sub-Saharan Africa: A
comprehensive scoping review. Autism Research.
10,723-749. doi: 10.1002/aur.1766.
[12] Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger
LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. (2004) Autism and abnormal
development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci. 24,
9228-9231.
[13] Vargas DL, Nascimbene C, Krishnan C, Zimmerman AW,
Pardo CA (2005) Neuroglial activation and

Page 8
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
21
neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann
Neurol. 57, 67-81.
[14] Vojdani A, Campbell AW, Anyanwu E, Kashanian A, Bock
K, Vojdani E (2002) Antibodies to neuron-specific antigens
in children with autism: possible cross-reaction with
encephalitogenic proteins from milk, Chlamydia
pneumoniae and Streptococcus group A. J Neuroimmunol.
129, 168-177.
[15] Singer HS, Morris CM, Williams PN, Yoon DY, Hong JJ,
Zimmerman AW (2006) Antibrain antibodies in children
with autism and their unaffected siblings. J Neuroimmunol.
178, 149-155.
[16] Bacchelli E, Maestrini E (2006) Autism spectrum disorders:
molecular genetic advances. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med
Genet. 142C, 13-23.
[17] Yang ZL, Sun GL (2016) Research advances in candidate
genes for autism spectrum disorder. Zhongguo Dang Dai Er
Ke Za Zhi. 18, 282-7.
[18] Shaw CA, Sheth S, Li D, Tomljenovic L (2014) Etiology of
autism spectrum disorders: Genes, environment, or both?
OA Autism. 2, 11
[19] Brewer JM (2006) (How) do aluminium adjuvants work?
Immunol Lett. 102, 10-15.
[20] Lotter V (1980) Cross cultural perspectives on childhood
autism. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics 26, 131 -133
[21] Bakare MO, Munir KM (2011) AutismSpectrum Disorders
in Africa, A Comprehensive Book on Autism Spectrum
Disorders, Dr. Mohammad-Reza Mohammadi (Ed.), ISBN:
978-953-307-494-8, InTechOpen.
[22] Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y.J., Kim, Y.S., Kauchali, S.,
Marcin, C., et al. (2012) Global prevalence of autism and
other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Research.
5, 160-179.
[23] Wing, L., Potter, D. (2002) The epidemiology of autistic
spectrum disorders: is the prevalence rising? Mental
retardation and developmental disabilities research
reviews.8, 151-61.
[24] Gillberg, C., Wing, L. (1999) Autism: not an extremely rare
disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 99, 399–406
[25] Fombonne, E. (2005) Epidemiology of autistic disorder and
other pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry.36:272–281.
[26] Kogan, M.D., Blumberg, S.J., Schieve, L.A., Boyle, C.A.,
Perrin, J.M., Ghandour, R.M., et al. (2007) Prevalence of
parent-reported diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder
among children in the US, 2007. Pediatrics 124, 1395-403.
[27] Centres for Disease Control and Prevention. Prevalence of
autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years –
autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network,
11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summ 2012;
61:1–19
[28] Zablotsky, B., Black, L.I., Maenner, M.J., Schieve, L.A.,
Blumberg, S.J. (2015) Estimated Prevalence of Autism and
Other Developmental Disabilities Following Questionnaire
Changes in the 2014 National Health Interview Survey.
National Health Statistics Report. 13(87), 1-20.
[29] Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Chandler S, Loucas T,
Meldrum D, Charman T (2006) Prevalence of disorders of
the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in
South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project
(SNAP). Lancet. 368(9531), 210-5.
[30] The National Autistic Society (2014) What is Autism?
www.autism.org.uk/about-autism.
[31] Saraceno B, Saxena S (2002) Mental health resources in the
world: results from Project Atlas of the WHO,” World
Psychiatry 1, 40-44.
[32] Cubells JF (2013) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders
in China. Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry. 25, 176–177.
[33] Al-Ansari, A.M., Ahmed, M.M. (2013) Epidemiology of
autistic disorder in Bahrain: prevalence and obstetric and
familial characteristics. Mediterrenean Health Journal. 19,
769-774.
[34] Al-Farsi, Y.M., Al-Sharbati, M.M., Al-Farsi, O.A.,
Al-Shafaee, M.S., Brooks, D.R., Waly, MI. (2011) Brief
report: Prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in the
Sultanate of Oman. Journal of Autism Developmental
Disorders. 41, 821-825.
[35] Al-Salehi, S.M., Al-Hifthy, E.H., Ghaziuddin, M. (2009)
Autism in Saudi Arabia: presentation, clinical correlates and
comorbidity. Transcultural Psychiatry. 46, 340-347.
[36] Eapen, V., Mabrouk, A.A., Zoubeidi, T., Yunis, F. (2007)
Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders in
preschool children in the UAE. Journal of Tropical
Pediatrics. 53, 202-205.
[37] Salhia, H.O., Al-Nasser, L.A., Taher, L.S., Al-Khathaami,
A.M., El-Metwally, A.A. (2014) Systemic review of the
epidemiology of autism in Arab Gulf countries.
Neurosciences (Riyadh). 19(4), 291-6.
[38] Ouellette-Kuntz H, Coo H, Yu CT, Lewis ME, Dewey D,
Hennessey PE, Jackman PD, Breitenbach MM, Holden JJ
(2012) Status report - National Epidemiologic Database for
the Study of Autism in Canada (NEDSAC). National
Epidemiological Database for the Study of Autism in
Canada NEDSAC; www.nedsac.ca Chronic Dis Inj Can. 32,
84-9.
[39] Paula CS, Ribeiro SH, Fombonne E, Mercadante MT. (2011)
Brief Report: Prevalence of Pervasive Developmental
Disorder in Brazil: A Pilot Study. Journal of Autism
Developmental Disorders. 41, 1738–1742. doi:
10.1007/s10803-011-1200-6.
[40] Dekkers LMS, Groot NA, Mosquera END, Z��iga IPA,
Delfos MF (2015) Prevalence of Autism Spectrum
Disorders in Ecuador: A Pilot Study in Quito J Autism Dev
Disord. 45(12): 4165–4173.
doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2559-6
[41] Fombonne E, Marcin C, Manero AC, Bruno R, Diaz C,
Villalobos M, Ramsay K, Nealy B (2016), Prevalence of
Autism Spectrum Disorders in Guanajuato, Mexico: The
Leon survey J Autism Dev Disord Volume 46, 1669–1685.
[42] WHO, World Health Organisation. (2013) Autism Spectrum
Disorders & Other Developmental Disorders. From Raising
Awareness to Building Capacity. Geneva, Switzerland.

Page 9
22
Global Data on Autism Spectrum Disorders Prevalence: A Review of Facts, Fallacies and Limitations
[43] Newton CR, Chugani DC (2013) The continuing role of
ICNA in Africa: how to tackle autism? Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology. 55, 488–489.
[44] Lagunju IA, Bella-Awusah TT, Omigbodun OO (2014)
Autistic disorder in Nigeria: profile and challenges to
management. Epilepsy Behav. 39:126-9.
[45] Bakare MO, Igwe MN, Odinka PC, Iteke O (2011)
Neuropsychiatric diagnosis and psychotropic medication
prescription patterns in a mental hospital-based child and
adolescent psychiatric service in Nigeria. Journal of Health
Care for the Poor and Underserved. 22, 751-5.
[46] Kakooza-Mwesige A, Ssebyala K, Karamagi C, Kiguli S,
Smith K, Anderson MC, et al. (2014) Adaptation of the "ten
questions" to screen for autism and other
neurodevelopmental disorders in Uganda. Autism.
18,447-57.
[47] Kalra V, Seth R, Sapra S (2005) Autism --experiences in a
tertiary care hospital. Indian Journal of Pediatrics. 72,
227-30.
[48] Divan G, Vajaratkar V. Desai MU, Strik-Lievers L, Patel V
(2012) Challenges, coping strategies, and unmet needs of
families with a child with autism spectrum disorder in Goa,
India. Autism Research. 5, 190-200.
[49] Malhi P, Singhi PA (2014) A retrospective study of toddlers
with autism spectrum disorder: Clinical and developmental
profile Annals of the Indian Academy of Neurology. 17,
25-9.
[50] Raina SK, Chander V, Bhardwaj AK, Kumar D, Sharma S,
Kashyap V, et al (2017) Prevalence of Autism Spectrum
Disorder among Rural, Urban, and Tribal Children (1-10
Years of Age). Journal of Neuroscience in Rural Practice. 8,
368-374.
[51] Raina SK, Kashyap V, Bhardwaj AK, Kumar D, Chander V
(2015) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders among
children (1-10 years of age) - findings of a mid-term report
from Northwest India. Journal of Postgraduate Medicine 61,
243-6.
[52] van Balkom IDC, Bresnahan M, Vogtl�nder MF, van
Hoeken D, Minderaa RB, Susser E, Hoek HW (2009)
Prevalence of treated autism spectrum disorders in ArubaJ
Neurodev Disord. 1, 197–204.
doi:10.1007/s11689-009-9011-1
[53] Gillberg C, Schaumann H, Gillberg IC (1995) Autism in
immigrants: children born in Sweden to mothers born in
Uganda. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 39, 141
– 144.
[54] Gillberg IC, Gillberg C (1996) Autism in immigrants: a
population-based study from Swedish rural and urban areas.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 40, 24 – 31.
[55] Barnevick-Olsson M, Gillberg C, Fernell E (2008)
Prevalence of au tism in children born to Somali parents
living in Sweden: a brief report. Developmental Medicine
and Child Neurology.50, 598 – 601.
[56] Mandell DS, Ittenbach RF, Levy SE, Pinto-Martin JA (2007)
Disparities in diagnoses received prior to a diagnosis of
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders. 37, 1795-802.
[57] Bello-Mojeed MA, Ogun OC, Omigbodun OO, Adewuya
OA (2010) Late identification of autistic disorder in Nigeria:
an illustration with 2 case reports. Nigerian Journal of
Psychiatry 9, 31–35.
[58] Nevison CD (2014) A comparison of temporal trends in
United States autism prevalence to trends in suspected
environmental factors. Environmental Health 13, 73. doi:
10.1186/1476-069X-13-73.
[59] Sharma JR, Arieff Z, Gameeldien H, Davids M, Kaur M,
Van Der Merwe L (2013) Association analysis of two
single-nucleotide polymorphisms of the RELN gene with
autism in the South African population. Genetic Testing and
Molecular Biomarkers. 17, 93–98.
[60] Schlebusch L, Samuels AE, Dada S 92016) South African
families raising children with autism spectrum disorders:
relationship between family routines, cognitive appraisal
and family quality of life. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research. 60, 412–423.
[61] Bello-Mojeed MA, Omigbodun OO, Bakare MO, Adewuya
AO (2017) Pattern of impairments and late diagnosis of
autism spectrum disorder among a sub-Saharan African
clinical population of children in Nigeria. Global Mental
Health (Cambridge). 21, 4:e5.
[62] Hansen SN, Schendel DE, Parner ET (2015) Explaining the
increase in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders the
proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices
JAMA Pediatrics 169,56-62.
[63] Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, Phillips J, Cohen B,
Torigoe T, et al (2011) Genetic heritability and shared
environmental factors among twin pairs with autism.
Archives of General Psychiatry. 68:1095–1102.
[64] Dufault R, Lukiw WJ, Crider R, Schnoll R, Wallinga D,
Deth R (2012) A macroepigenetic approach to identify
factors responsible for the autism epidemic in the United
States.
Clinical
Epigenetics.
4,
6.
doi:
10.1186/1868-7083-4-6.
[65] Grabrucker AM (2013) Environmental factors in autism.
Frontiers in Psychiatry. 4, 1–13.
[66] Velasquez-Manoff M (2015) An Immune Disorder at the
Root of Autism,” The New York Times,
http://www.nytimes.com/2012/08/26/opinion/
[67] Parker W (2013) “Helminthic Therapy: Reconstituting the
depleted biome to prevent immune disorders,” The
Environmental Illness Resource,
http://www.ei-resource.org/articles/candida-and-gut-dysbio
sis-articles/.
[68] Taylor B, Miller E, Farrington CP, Petropoulos MC,
Favot-Mayaud I, Li J, et al (1999) Autism and measles,
mumps, and rubella vaccine: No epidemiologic evidence for
a causal association. Lancet. 353, 2026–9.
[69] Dales L, Hammer SJ, Smith NJ (2001) Time trends in autism
and in MMR immunization coverage in California. Journal
of the American Medical Association. 285, 1183–5.
[70] Whitacre CC (2001) Sex differences in autoimmune disease.
Nature Immunology. 2(9), 777-80.
[71] Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Pankratz VS,
Weaver AL, Weber KJ, et al (2002) How common is

Page 10
Universal Journal of Clinical Medicine 5(2): 14-23, 2017
23
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Incidence in a
population-based birth cohort in Rochester, Minnesota.
Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 156, 217-24
[72] Viding E, Spinath FM, Price TS, Bishop DV, Dale PS,
Plomin R (2004) Genetic and environmental influence on
language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and
opposite-sex twins. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry. 45, 315-25.
[73] Pinborough-Zimmerman J, Satterfield R, Miller J, Bilder D,
Hossain S, McMahon W (2007) Communication disorders:
prevalence and comorbid intellectual disability, autism, and
emotional/behavioral disorders. American Journal of Speech
and Language Pathology. 16, 359-67.
[74] Werling DM, Geschwind DH (2013) Sex differences in
autism spectrum disorders Current Opinion in Neurology. 26,
146–153.
[75] Halladay, A.K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J.N., Daniels, A.M.,
Koenig, K., Palmer, K., et al. (2015) Sex and gender
differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing
evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority.
Molecular Autism 6, 36. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y
[76] Hirvikoski T, Mittendorfer-Rutz E, Boman M., Larsson, H.,
Lichtenstein, P., Bolte, S. (2016) Premature mortality in
autism spectrum disorder The British Journal of Psychiatry
208, 232–238. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.160192.
[77] Mouridsen SE, Bronnum-Hansen H, Rich B, Isager T (2008)
Mortality and causes of death in autism spectrum disorders:
an update. Autism 12, 403–14.