Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Dec 20;12(12):e0189019.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189019. eCollection 2017.

Neural mechanisms underlying sensitivity to reverse-phi motion in the fly

Affiliations

Neural mechanisms underlying sensitivity to reverse-phi motion in the fly

Aljoscha Leonhardt et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Optical illusions provide powerful tools for mapping the algorithms and circuits that underlie visual processing, revealing structure through atypical function. Of particular note in the study of motion detection has been the reverse-phi illusion. When contrast reversals accompany discrete movement, detected direction tends to invert. This occurs across a wide range of organisms, spanning humans and invertebrates. Here, we map an algorithmic account of the phenomenon onto neural circuitry in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Through targeted silencing experiments in tethered walking flies as well as electrophysiology and calcium imaging, we demonstrate that ON- or OFF-selective local motion detector cells T4 and T5 are sensitive to certain interactions between ON and OFF. A biologically plausible detector model accounts for subtle features of this particular form of illusory motion reversal, like the re-inversion of turning responses occurring at extreme stimulus velocities. In light of comparable circuit architecture in the mammalian retina, we suggest that similar mechanisms may apply even to human psychophysics.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: HE is an employee of Google Inc. The company provided support in the form of salaries for author HE, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific role of this author is articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section. This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. The neural architecture of motion detection in the fruit fly.
a Stylized rendering of neuropils and cells involved in generating local and global motion signals in the ON-selective T4 pathway. b Corresponding schematic for the OFF-selective T5 pathway. Briefly, optical signals are transduced by photoreceptors in the retina, split into ON and OFF signals at the lamina level, processed by an array of interneurons in the medulla, and become direction selective through interactions on T4 and T5 dendrites. Finally, lobula plate tangential cells (LPTCs) select and pool appropriate local signals from four directionally tuned layers to generate complex flow field sensitivities.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Spectral properties of translating motion stimuli.
a Space-time plot of a one-dimensional noise pattern moving to the right at constant speed (v = 60° s-1). b Magnitude spectrum of translating noise, with darkness indicating larger frequency contribution. For rigid motion toward the right, components cluster around a line connecting first and third quadrant (in green) via the origin, with slope indicating true velocity. Little magnitude is assigned to frequencies in the quadrants signaling leftward motion (in brown). c Left, space-time plot of a discretized square-wave grating (v = 60° s-1, λ = 60°, step width = 2°). Right, quantification of energy indicating right- and leftward motion, respectively. Energy was simply averaged within quadrants color-coded above. Energy distribution is not affected by half-wave rectification (i.e., zeroing either components below or above mean luminance for ON or OFF respectively). d Corresponding illustration and quantification for discretized reverse-phi stimulus. Energy distribution reverses for full-wave stimulus but signals veridical direction when rectification is applied (ON or OFF).
Fig 3
Fig 3. Reverse-phi responses in tethered walking flies.
a Average turning response for phi condition at three different stimulus velocities and two spatial wavelengths, indicated by panel title and line color. Data are from T4/T5 control flies. Control flies (N = 12) rotate syndirectionally with stimulus. Gray shaded area indicates stimulation period. b Summary statistics for phi condition (average from 1.5 to 3.0 s after trial onset). c Average turning response for reverse-phi condition. Control responses (N = 10) are inverted at low velocities. At high velocities and depending on spatial wavelength, the optomotor response reverses again. The inset depicts the first 500 ms (see black line in main panel) of the response to stimulation with a pattern velocity of 256° s-1 (λ = 90°), highlighting temporally biphasic dynamics. d Summary statistics for reverse-phi condition. Shaded areas around curves and bars around points indicate 95% confidence intervals. See Materials and Methods for details on behavioral experiments.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Local motion-sensitive cells T4 and T5 are required for reverse-phi responses in walking flies.
a Average turning response for phi condition at two different stimulus velocities, indicated by panel title (λ = 90° and step width = 4° throughout figure). Here, control flies (N = 12/12 for shibirets and T4/T5 controls) turn with the direction of the stimulus. This behavior is abolished in T4/T5 block flies (N = 11). Gray shaded area indicates stimulation period. b Summary statistics for phi condition (average from 1.5 to 3.0 s after trial onset). c Walking speed statistics (averaged as before) for phi condition. No genotype exhibits impaired locomotion. d Average turning response for reverse-phi condition. Control responses (N = 11/10 for shibirets and T4/T5 controls) are inverted at low velocities. At high velocities, re-inversion occurs. Turning is fully abolished in T4/T5 block flies (N = 10). e Summary statistics for reverse-phi condition. f Walking speed statistics for reverse-phi condition. Shaded areas around curves and bars around points indicate 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks mark significant differences between T4/T5 block flies and both controls (Student’s t-test, P < 0.001). Note that whiskers show the full sample range. See Materials and Methods for details on behavioral experiments and statistics.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Motion-sensitive tangential cells show selectively inverted responses upon reverse-phi stimulation.
a-b Average membrane potential of lobula plate tangential cells (N = 23, pooled across 8 horizontal and 15 vertical system cells, with accordingly oriented direction of movement) in wild-type flies. Responses were recorded at 2 kHz and down-sampled to 50 Hz for presentation. Gray shaded area indicates stimulation period (λ = 45°, v = 128° s-1, step width = 6°). Shading around traces shows bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. a Phi stimulation. Cells depolarize for motion in preferred direction (PD) and hyperpolarize weakly for motion in the opposite null direction (ND). b Reverse-phi stimulation. The PD response is abolished, whereas the ND response inverts. c Summary statistics (averaged across initial 2 s post-onset). Note that whiskers show the full sample range. See Materials and Methods for details on electrophysiology experiments.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Architecture of four- and two-quadrant motion detection models.
a Signal flow for a single four-quadrant, non-rectifying elementary motion detector (HP: first-order high-pass filter, LP: first-order low-pass filter, X: multiplication). Quadrants are possible combinations of positive (ON) and negative (OFF) signal contrast. Properties of rectification are displayed as schematic transfer functions (non-rectified in green, ON in red, OFF in blue). If no rectification between ON and OFF occurs, all four possible correlations (ON-ON, ON-OFF, OFF-ON, OFF-OFF) are directly computed. b Signal flow for two-quadrant, rectified elementary motion detector (DC: direct unfiltered input component). Here, signals of equal sign (quadrants ON-ON and OFF-OFF) are correlated.
Fig 7
Fig 7. A two-quadrant model admitting constant luminance accounts for behavioral and neural responses.
a-b Velocity tuning of two model architectures (4Q: non-rectified four-quadrant detector; 2Q: rectified two-quadrant detector with constant luminance contribution, DC). All detectors had identical base parameters (τHP = 250 ms; τLP = 50 ms; DC = 0 or 10% for 4Q or 2Q, respectively; receptor distance = 4°). Simulations were carried out at time steps of 1.0 ms over 10 s of stimulation per velocity. Lines represent time- and space-averaged responses of 60 detectors covering 240° of visual space, normalized to +/-1 maximum/minimum per panel. Black lines: Responses to phi stimulation across 20 velocities on logarithmic scale (λ = 90°, step width = 4°). Gray lines: Responses to corresponding reverse-phi stimulation. Only the rectified two-quadrant model replicates both response reversal and re-inversion at high velocities, as indicated by experimental results. c Velocity tuning for rectified 2Q detector with DC set to zero. Reverse-phi response now indicates veridical direction across full range. d Velocity tuning for 4Q detector with DC level set to 10%. e Velocity tuning for 2Q detector when flicker and motion updates are out of phase. f-i Parameter scans for low- and high-pass time constants of 4Q and 2Q detectors. Arrows indicate default setting. The respective other time constant was kept at its default value. Qualitative tuning features appear to not depend on choice of parameters. See Materials and Methods for further details on simulations.
Fig 8
Fig 8. Detailed spatiotemporal tuning of 2Q detector.
a Phi velocity tuning curves of 2Q detector for different spatial wavelengths. Response optima shift toward higher velocities with increased λ as predicted for Reichardt-type motion detectors. b Velocity tuning for reverse-phi condition. Here, response peaks remain fixed. Spatial wavelength affects magnitude of optimal response and modulates inversion at high pattern speeds, in line with behavioral findings. c Temporally resolved response of a summed array of 2Q detectors in response to phi motion (λ = 90°). Traces were lightly low-pass filtered (τ = 200 ms) to approximate temporal integration during visuomotor transformation. Regardless of velocity, the steady-state response indicates veridical motion. d Resolved 2Q response to reverse-phi stimulation. At low velocities the response is consistently inverted; for fast velocities, a brief inversion is rapidly followed by positive steady-state output (compare behavioral data in Fig 3C). All parameters are set to their default values. Note that we normalize response peaks per panel. See Materials and Methods for further details on simulations.
Fig 9
Fig 9. Decoupling flicker and motion components.
a Turning responses of wild-type flies (N = 11) for phi-type stimuli in which flicker and motion frequencies were varied independently (equivalent stimulus velocities are obtained through multiplication by the spatial step width of 4°; λ = 90°). Response sign and magnitude are a complex function of both parameters. b Response matrix for normalized data from a (with maximum set to 1.0). The first row and the diagonal correspond exactly to previously used phi and reverse-phi stimuli, respectively. Note that values are clipped at 0.5. c Response matrix for a four-quadrant detector using the same stimulus parameters as in the behavioral experiment. Critically, this model does not predict positive responses below the diagonal. d Response matrix for a two-quadrant detector. Crucial features like positive below-diagonal responses are captured faithfully. e Direct comparison of diagonal values equivalent to the standard reverse-phi stimulus. Bars around points indicate 95% confidence intervals. See Materials and Methods for details on behavioral experiments and statistics.
Fig 10
Fig 10. Pathway-specific responses of 2Q model.
a Velocity tuning curves for reverse-phi pattern (λ = 90°) before summation of ON- and OFF-specific channels (ON subunit in red, OFF subunit in blue). Only the ON pathway shows inversion and re-inversion; the OFF pathway consistently indicates physical motion direction. b Reverse-phi velocity tuning of the ON subunit as a function of DC contribution. The balance between veridical motion signal and inversion depends on the presence of luminance information at the input; for intermediate levels, positive and negative responses are possible, depending on the particular stimulus velocity. Arrow indicates the default DC parameter of 10%. Values are normalized to a maximum absolute response of one. c Equivalent tuning for the OFF subunit. At the standard level of 10%, only positive responses occur. For negative DC, a similar picture as for the ON subunit emerges. Parameters except for DC level are set to their default values. See Materials and Methods for further details on simulations.
Fig 11
Fig 11. Reverse-phi responses of T4 and T5 dendrites as revealed by in vivo calcium imaging.
a Representative two-photon image of mean GCaMP6m activity within field of view. Only cells targeting the upward-tuned layer 3 of the lobula plate are labeled by the driver line, allowing for isolation of T4 or T5 dendrites through appropriate placement of regions of interest on either medulla or lobula dendrites, respectively. b Calcium responses in T4 dendrites (N = 9 flies) for phi stimulus at two velocities (λ = 60°, step width = 4°). Units are strongly tuned to motion in the preferred (upward) direction. c Corresponding T4 responses for reverse-phi motion. Depending on velocity, either PD or ND motion evokes stronger response. d Summary of T4 responses, quantified as difference between PD and ND signals (time-averaged between 1 and 4 s after stimulation onset). Calcium activity closely matches behavioral and model results. e-g Corresponding results for T5 dendrites. Phi responses resemble those measured in T4. For reverse-phi, a smaller inversion is found. Shaded areas around curves and bars around points indicate 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks show significant differences from zero for reverse-phi responses (Student’s t-test, P < 0.05); no tests were performed for phi signals. See Materials and Methods for further details on imaging procedures.
Fig 12
Fig 12. Behavioral responses to reverse-phi stimulation in pathway-specific block flies.
a Summary statistics for reverse-phi condition (average from 1.5 to 3.0 s after trial onset; λ = 90°) in T4 block flies (N = 14) and associated controls (N = 12/18 for TNT and T4 controls). b Summary statistics for reverse-phi condition in T5 block flies (N = 15) and associated controls (N = 12/12 for TNT and T5 controls). In both T4 and T5 block flies, turning responses are reduced and re-inversion disappears. Bars around points indicate 95% confidence intervals c Secondary reverse-phi experiment with increased maximum velocity. For T4 block flies (N = 12), high-speed responses are again re-inverted and in line with TNT control flies (N = 11). In T5 block flies (N = 13), high-speed re-inversion remains abolished. Other stimulus parameters match experiments in Fig 4. Lines represent 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks indicate significant differences between T4 or T5 block flies and both appropriate controls (Student’s t-test, P < 0.01). See Materials and Methods for further details on behavioral experiments and statistics.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Borst A. Fly visual course control: behaviour, algorithms and circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15: 590–599. doi: 10.1038/nrn3799 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Borst A, Helmstaedter M. Common circuit design in fly and mammalian motion vision. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18: 1067–1076. doi: 10.1038/nn.4050 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Anstis SM. Phi movement as a subtraction process. Vision Research. 1970;10: 1411–1430. - PubMed
    1. Eichner H, Joesch M, Schnell B, Reiff DF, Borst A. Internal structure of the fly elementary motion detector. Neuron. 2011;70: 1155–1164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.028 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Clark DA, Bursztyn L, Horowitz MA, Schnitzer MJ, Clandinin TR. Defining the computational structure of the motion detector in Drosophila. Neuron. 2011;70: 1165–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.023 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Grants and funding

This work was supported by the Max-Planck-Society. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Google Inc. provided support in the form of salaries for author HE, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific role of this author is articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.

LinkOut - more resources