Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Sep;29(3):658-65.
doi: 10.1037/a0037234.

Role of sleep continuity and total sleep time in executive function across the adult lifespan

Affiliations

Role of sleep continuity and total sleep time in executive function across the adult lifespan

Kristine A Wilckens et al. Psychol Aging. 2014 Sep.

Abstract

The importance of sleep for cognition in young adults is well established, but the role of habitual sleep behavior in cognition across the adult life span remains unknown. We examined the relationship between sleep continuity and total sleep time as assessed with a sleep-detection device, and cognitive performance using a battery of tasks in young (n = 59, mean age = 23.05) and older (n = 53, mean age = 62.68) adults. Across age groups, higher sleep continuity was associated with better cognitive performance. In the younger group, higher sleep continuity was associated with better working memory and inhibitory control. In the older group, higher sleep continuity was associated with better inhibitory control, memory recall, and verbal fluency. Very short and very long total sleep time was associated with poorer working memory and verbal fluency, specifically in the younger group. Total sleep time was not associated with cognitive performance in any domains for the older group. These findings reveal that sleep continuity is important for executive function in both young and older adults, but total sleep time may be more important for cognition in young adults.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Scatter plots of the linear bivariate relationships between WASO (x axis) and each cognitive domain (y axis) for young (black) and older participants (gray). Trend lines reflect linear relationships. Significant linear relationships are denoted, p < 0.05 = *, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.001 = ***
Figure 2
Figure 2
Scatter plots of the bivariate relationships between TST (x axis) and each cognitive domain (y axis) for young (black) and older participants (gray). Trend lines reflect quadratic relationships. Significant quadratic relationships are denoted, p < 0.05 = *, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.001 = ***

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Anderson C, Horne JA. Prefrontal cortex: links between low frequency delta EEG in sleep and neuropsychological performance in healthy, older people. Psychophysiology. 2003;40(3):349–357. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00038. - PubMed
    1. Baldo JV, Shimamura AP. Frontal Lobes and Memory. In: Baddeley, Kopelman, editors. Handbook of Memory Disorders. 2nd Edition John Wiley & Co.; London: 2002.
    1. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Schneider JL, Cauley JA, Hillier TA, Stone KL. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: the study of osteoporotic fractures. The Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2006;61(4):405–410. doi: 61/4/405 [pii] - PubMed
    1. Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44(1):195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006. - PubMed
    1. Buysse D, Browman K, Monk T, Reynolds C, 3rd, Fasiczka A, Kupfer D. Napping and 24-hour sleep/wake patterns in healthy elderly and young adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1992;40(8):779. - PubMed

Publication types