Skip to main content
Log in

Pathogen Prevalence, Group Bias, and Collectivism in the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been argued that people in areas with high pathogen loads will be more likely to avoid outsiders, to be biased in favor of in-groups, and to hold collectivist and conformist values. Cross-national studies have supported these predictions. In this paper we provide new pathogen codes for the 186 cultures of the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample and use them, together with existing pathogen and ethnographic data, to try to replicate these cross-national findings. In support of the theory, we found that cultures in high pathogen areas were more likely to socialize children toward collectivist values (obedience rather than self-reliance). There was some evidence that pathogens were associated with reduced adult dispersal. However, we found no evidence of an association between pathogens and our measures of group bias (in-group loyalty and xenophobia) or intergroup contact.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barry, H., Josephson, L., Lauer, E., & Marshall, C. (1976). Traits inculcated in childhood: cross-cultural codes 5. Ethnology, 15(1), 83–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barth, F. (1969). Ethnic groups and boundaries: The social organization of culture difference. Boston: Waveland Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. (2001). Constructing frames of reference: An analytical method for archaeological theory building using ethnographic and environmental data sets. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. (2001). Ethnocentrism and xenophobia: a cross-cultural study. Current Anthropology, 42(5), 760–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. (2012). In-group loyalty or out-group avoidance? Isolating the links between pathogens and in-group assortative sociality. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J., & Duncan, L. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, E., & Russell, P. (1964). Craig and Faust’s clinical parasitology. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincher, C., & Thornhill, R. (2008a). Assortative sociality, limited dispersal, infectious disease and the genesis of the global pattern of religion diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1651), 2587–2594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fincher, C., & Thornhill, R. (2008b). A parasite-driven wedge: infectious diseases may explain language and other biodiversity. Oikos, 117(9), 1289–1297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fincher, C., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: the cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 39–59. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11001774.

  • Fincher, C., Thornhill, R., Murray, D., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1640), 1279–1285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, M., Raver, J., Nishii, L., Leslie, L., Lun, J., Lim, B., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, B. (1989). Cross-cultural patterns in the training of children: an evolutionary perspective. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103(4), 311–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, B. (1990). Marriage systems and pathogen stress in human societies. American Zoologist, 30(2), 325–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Low, B. S. (1994). Pathogen intensity cross-culturally: SCCS. World Cultures, 8(2).

  • Moody, J. (2001). Race, school integration, and friendship segregation in America. The American Journal of Sociology, 107(3), 679–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1971). Measurement of cultural complexity. Ethnology, 12, 379–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G., & White, D. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 8(4), 329–369. Updated at http://128.195.148.16/~drwhite/pub/SCCS1969.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P., & Wilson, S. F. (1972). Settlement patterns and community organization: cross cultural codes 3. Ethnology, 11, 254–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G., Wilson, S., & Frederick, V. (1978). World distribution of theories of illness. Ethnology, 17(4), 449–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D., & Schaller, M. (2010). Historical prevalence of infectious diseases within 230 geopolitical regions: a tool for investigating origins of culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(1), 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navarrete, C., & Fessler, D. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: the effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 270–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. (2007). E pluribus unum: civic engagement in a diverse and changing society. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30(2), 137–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodenwaldt, E., & Bader, R. E. (1961). World-atlas of epidemic diseases (1952–1961). Hamburg: Falk-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. (1983). Political decision making and conflict: additional cross-cultural codes and scales. Ethnology, 22, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute (2012). Base SAS(R) 9.3 procedures guide: Statistical procedures. SAS Institute.

  • Schaller, M., & Duncan, L. (2007). The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippell (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp. 293–307). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, M., & Murray, D. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, M., & Murray, D. (2010). Infectious diseases and the evolution of cross-cultural differences. In M. Schaller (Ed.), Evolution, culture, and the human mind (pp. 243–256). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipley, B. (2002). Cause and correlation in Biology: A users guide to path analysis, structural equations and causal inference. Cambridge University Press.

  • Simmons, J. S., Whayne, T. F., Anderson, G. W., & Horack, H. M. (1944). Global epidemiology: A geography of disease and sanitation. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skitka, L. (2005). Patriotism or nationalism? Understanding post-September 11, 2001, flag-display behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(10), 1995–2011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., Fincher, C., Murray, D., & Schaller, M. (2010). Zoonotic and non-zoonotic diseases in relation to human personality and societal values: support for the parasite-stress model. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(2), 151–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, J. W. M. (1989). Climate data from weather stations. World Cultures, 1(1), 179–199.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Our greatest debt is to Damian Murray, for training us in the use of the pathogen sources he used for his cross-national historical pathogen codes. We thank him and Mark Schaller for their encouragement with the project and help with the literature. We are grateful to Bobbi Low for both her own insightful work and her consistent encouragement of our own efforts. We also thank Ryan Bohlander for his exploratory data analysis and persistence in the face of negative results, and Carol Ember and our anonymous reviewers for very helpful suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth Cashdan.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(RTF 95.9 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cashdan, E., Steele, M. Pathogen Prevalence, Group Bias, and Collectivism in the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample. Hum Nat 24, 59–75 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9159-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9159-3

Keywords

Navigation