Skip to main content
Log in

Relations Among Joint Attention, Amount of Intervention and Language Gain in Autism

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the unconditional and conditional relations between amount of intervention and language development in children with autism. Joint attention skills were proposed as child characteristics that might moderate this relation. The results replicated previous findings that better joint attention skills were associated with greater language development. The results further indicated that the relation between amount of intervention and gain in language age was conditional; it depended upon the child's ability to respond to bids for joint attention from others and initial language skills. The current study demonstrated the utility of employing characteristics of children as moderators of relations between interventions and developmental outcomes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
EUR 32.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or Ebook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Adamson, L.B., & McArthur, D. (1995). Joint attention, affect, and culture.In C. Moore & P.J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp.205–222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Pub-lications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L.B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant inter-action.Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D.A. (1993). Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P.A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In R.M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp.993–1028). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Evans, G.W. (2000). Developmental sci-ence in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Social Development, 9, 115–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in com-mon is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual atten-tion in infancy.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cogni-tion, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63.

  • Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Joint attention, cultural learning, and language acquisition: Implications for children with autism. In A.M. Wetherby & B.M. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders: A transactional developmental per-spective (pp.31–54). Baltimore, MD: Paul H.Brookes Pub-lishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, L.M. (1996). Measurement of change in research on aging: Old and new issues from an individual growth per-spective.In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of psychology of aging, Fourth edition, San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corkum, V., & Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In C. Moore & P.J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp.61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipek, P.A., Accardo, P.J., Baranek, G.T., Cook, E.H., Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., Gravel, J.S., Johnson, C.P., Kallen, R.J., Levy, S.E., Minshew, N.J., Prizant, B.M., Rapin, I., Rogers, S.J., Stone, W.L., Teplin, S., Tuchman, R.F., & Volkmar, F.R. (1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders.Journal of Autism & Develop-mental Disorders, 29, 439–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabriels, R.L., Hill, D.E., Pierce, R.A., Rogers, S.J., & Wehner, B. (2001). Predictors of treatment outcome in young children with autism.Autism, 5, 407–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution: The gen-esis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R.A. (1996). Describing relationships.In A.E. Auhagen & M. von Salisch (Eds.), The diversity of human relationships (pp.7–13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, B.M., Willen, J.D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult's eyes trig-ger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Freeman, S.F.N., & Paparella, T. (2001). Early intervention in autism: Joint attention and symbolic play.In L.M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in men-tal retardation: Autism (pp.207–237). San Diego, CA: Aca-demic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Yirmiya, N., & Mundy, P. (1993). Affec-tive development and communication in young children with autism. In A.P. Kaiser & D.B. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing chil-dren's communication: Research foundations for intervention (pp.201–222). Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H.Brookes Pub-lishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M.M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R.L., & Frea, W.D. (1993). Treatment of social beha-vior in autism through the modi cation of pivotal social skills.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 369–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R.L., Koegel, L.K., & Schreibman, L. (1991). Assessing and training parents in teaching pivotal behaviors.In R.J. Prinz (Ed.), Advances in behavioral assessment of children and families (pp.65–82). London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S.R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism.Develop-mental Psychology, 36, 261–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S.R., & Moore, C. (2001). The development of atten-tion and joint attention in children with autism.In J.A. Burack, T. Charman, et al. (Eds.), The development of aut-ism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp.105–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). The ADOS-G (Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnos-tic interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic inter-view for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.Journal of Autism and Developmen-tal Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., & Dunham, P.J. (Eds). (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Following the direc-tion of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 373–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E.M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism.Development & Psychopa-thology, 7, 63–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention and early social communication: Implications for research on interven-tion with autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-orders, 27, 653–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Nonverbal commu-nication, a. ective sharing, and intersubjectivity.Infant Behavior & Development, 15, 377–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Markus, J. (1997). On the nature of communication and language impairment in autism.Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 343–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism.Inter-national Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 139–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children.Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M.D., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social de cits of autism: The contribu-tion of non-verbal communication measures.Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., Sherman, T. (1987). Non-verbal communication and play correlates of language devel-opment in autistic children.Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 17, 349–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paparella, T. (2000). A developmental analysis of joint attention and requesting skills in young children with autism. Disserta-tion Abstracts International, 61 (2-A), 497. (UMI No. AAI9961641).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynell, J.K., & Gruber, C.P. (1990). Reynell developmental lan-guage scales. Los Angeles: Western Pychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D.R., & Willet, J.B. (1983). Demonstrating the reliabil-ity of the difference score in the measurement of change. Journal of Educational Measurement, 20, 335–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D.R., & Willet, J.B. (1985). Understanding correlates of change by modeling individual differences in growth.Psycho-metrika, 50, 203–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to adulthood.Semi-nars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scaife, M., & Bruner, J.S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant.Nature, 253, 265–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals —Revised. The Psychological Cor-poration.

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (February, 2003). Joint attention behav-iors in children with autism: Stability and change during the preschool years.Paper presented at the M.I.N.D.Institute Research Program: Annual Research Meeting, Davis, CA.

  • Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attachments in autistic children.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Ado-lescent Psychiatry, 28, 74–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, v-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J.A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M.D., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers.Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 647–656.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of chil-dren with autism predict the subsequent development of their children's communication.Journal of Autism & Devel-opmental Disorders, 32, 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J., & Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analy-sis.Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R.L., hagen, E.P., & Sattler, J.M. (1986). Stanford-Binet intelligence scale. Chicago: The Riverside Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development, 1, 67–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C.Moore & P.J.Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp.103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life.In M.Tomasello & E.Bates (Eds). Language development: The essential readings (pp. 111–128). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Herron, C. (1988). Down the Garden Path: Inducing and correcting overgeneralization errors in the foreign language classroom.Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 237–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J.A., & Sigman, M. (1984). The relation of play and sensorimotor behavior to language in the second year.Child Development, 55, 1448–1455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventner, A., Lord, C., & Shopler, E. (1992). A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 489–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modi cation procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 456–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and predictors of individual change over time.Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R.H., & Zimmerman, D.W. (1996). Are simple gain scores obsolete? Applied Psychological Measurement, 20, 59–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D.W. (1997). A geometric interpretation of the validity and reliability of difference scores.British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 50, 73–80.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bono, M.A., Daley, T. & Sigman, M. Relations Among Joint Attention, Amount of Intervention and Language Gain in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 34, 495–505 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x

Navigation