Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Early Expression of Blatant Dehumanization in Children and Its Association with Outgroup Negativity

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Dehumanization is observed in adults across cultures and is thought to motivate human violence. The age of its first expression remains largely untested. This research demonstrates that diverse representations of humanness, including a novel one, readily elicit blatant dehumanization in adults (N = 482) and children (aged 5–12; N = 150). Dehumanizing responses in both age groups are associated with support for outgroup inferiority. Similar to the link previously observed in adults, dehumanization by children is associated with a willingness to punish outgroup transgressors. These findings suggest that exposure to cultural norms throughout adolescence and adulthood are not required for the development of outgroup dehumanization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bain, P., Vaes, J., Kashima, Y., Haslam, N., & Guan, Y. (2012). Folk conceptions of humanness: Beliefs about distinctive and core human characteristics in Australia, Italy, and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 53–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2016). Moral disengagement: How people do harm and live with themselves. Worth, Macmillan

  • Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9(4), 253–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2017). The root of all cruelty? The New Yorker. https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2017/11/27/the-root-of-all-cruelty

  • Bohman, A., & Miklikowska, M. (2021). Does classroom diversity improve intergroup relations? Short-and long-term effects of classroom diversity for cross-ethnic friendships and anti-immigrant attitudes in adolescence. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(8), 1372–1390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruneau, E., Jacoby, N., Kteily, N., & Saxe, R. (2018). Denying humanity: The distinct neural correlates of blatant dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 1078–1093

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruneau, E., Szekeres, H., Kteily, N., Tropp, L. R., & Kende, A. (2020). Beyond dislike: Blatant dehumanization predicts teacher discrimination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 23(4), 560–577

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chas, A., Betancor, V., Delgado, N., & Rodríguez-Pérez, A. (2018). Children consider their own group to be more human than other social groups: Evidence from indirect and direct measures. Social Psychology, 49(3), 125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G., & Saxe, R. R. (2011). Us and them: Intergroup failures of empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 149–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cikara, M., Eberhardt, J. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). From agents to objects: Sexist attitudes and neural responses to sexualized targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 540–551

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cloutier, J., Ambady, N., Meagher, T., & Gabrieli, J. (2012). The neural substrates of person perception: Spontaneous use of financial and moral status knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2371–2376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2011). Social dominance-based threat reactions to immigrants in need of assistance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(2), 220–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014). Explaining dehumanization among children: The interspecies model of prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(1), 175–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dhont, K., Hodson, G., Loughnan, S., & Amiot, C. E. (2019). Rethinking human-animal relations: The critical role of social psychology. Group Process & Intergroup Relations, 22(6), 769–784

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dore, R. A., Hoffman, K. M., Lillard, A. S., & Trawalter, S. (2014). Children’s racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 218–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, T. F., Jones, S. C., Kaylor-Hughes, C. J., Wilkinson, I. D., Woodruff, P. W., Hunter, M. D., & Spence, S. A. (2011). Higher or lower? The functional anatomy of perceived allocentric social hierarchies. Neuroimage, 57(4), 1552–1560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M. J., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2001). Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Animal Cognition, 4(3), 269–280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., & Woods, V. (2021). Survival of the friendliest: Understanding our origins and rediscovering our common humanity. Random House

  • Harris, L. T. (2017). Invisible mind: Flexible social cognition and dehumanization. MIT Press

  • Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847–853

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N., & Loughman, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399–423. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Kteily, N., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Pratto, F., Henkel, K. E. … Stewart, A. L. (2015). The nature of social dominance orientation: Theorizing and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality using the new SDO7 scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 1003–1028

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodson, G., Crisp, R. J., Meleady, R., & Earle, M. (2018). Intergroup contact as an agent of cognitive liberalization. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(5), 523–548

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B. A., Boyette, A. H. … Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36), 14586–14591

  • Jardina, A., & Piston, S. (2021). The effects of dehumanizing attitudes about Black people on Whites’ voting decisions. British Journal of Political Science, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123421000089

  • Kteily, N., & Bruneau, E. (2017). Darker demons of our nature: The need to (re) focus attention on blatant forms of dehumanization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 487–494

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kteily, N., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., & Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 901–931. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000048

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kteily, N., Hodson, G., & Bruneau, E. (2016). They see us as less than human: Metadehumanization predicts intergroup conflict via reciprocal dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J., Goldstein, A., Influs, M., Masalha, S., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2016). Adolescents growing up amidst intractable conflict attenuate brain response to pain of outgroup. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(48), 13696–13701

  • Markowitz, D. M., & Slovic, P. (2020). Social, psychological, and demographic characteristics of dehumanization toward immigrants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(17), 9260–9269

  • Martin, J., Bennett, M., & Murray, W. S. (2008). A developmental study of the infrahumanization hypothesis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 153–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCallum, C. (2001). Gender and sociality in Amazonia: How real people are made. Berg

  • McLoughlin, N., Tipper, S. P., & Over, H. (2018). Young children perceive less humanness in outgroup faces. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12539

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond western, educated, industrial, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) psychology: measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678–701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H. (2021). Seven challenges for the dehumanization hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 3–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. T., Barreto, M., Karl, A., & Lawrence, N. (2019). Empathic responses are reduced to competitive but not non-competitive outgroups. Social neuroscience, 14(3), 345–358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, V. B., Villar, A. C., Rodríguez-Pérez, A., & Rodríguez, N. D. (2016). Infrahumanization in children: An evaluation of 70 terms relating to humanity. Psicothema, 28(1), 53–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. L. (2020). On inhumanity: Dehumanization and how to resist it. Oxford University Press

  • Sumner, W. G. (1906). Folkways: A study of the sociological importance of usages, manners, customs, mores, and morals. Ginn

  • Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press

  • Trounson, J. S., Critchley, C., & Pfeifer, J. E. (2015). Australian attitudes toward asylum seekers: Roles of dehumanization and social dominance theory. Social Behavior and Personality, 43(10), 1641–1655

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Noorden, T. H., Haselager, G. J., Cillessen, A. H., & Bukowski, W. M. (2014). Dehumanization in children: The link with moral disengagement in bullying and victimization. Aggressive Behavior, 40(4), 320–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vezzali, L., Hewstone, M., Capozza, D., Trifiletti, E., & Bernardo, G. A. D. (2017). Improving intergroup relations with extended contact among young children: Mediation by intergroup empathy and moderation by direct intergroup contact. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 27(1), 35–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viki, G. T., Osgood, D., & Phillips, S. (2013). Dehumanization and self-reported proclivity to torture prisoners of war. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 325–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilks, M., Caviola, L., Kahane, G., & Bloom, P. (2021). Children prioritize humans over animals less than adults do. Psychological Science, 32(1), 27–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. (2019). The goodness paradox: The strange relationship between virtue and violence in human evolution. Pantheon

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Zoo Atlanta for hosting our research, to the parents and children for participating and to Aleah Bowie, Erica Blair, Feruth Kidane, Julia Villegas, and Morgan Ferrans who helped collect the data. We thank Philip White and Zachary White who gave us advice on statistics. We also wish to thank Aleah Bowie, Michael Tomasello, Sarah Gaither and Ashley Jardina for invaluable comments they provided on an earlier version of the manuscript. Funding for this work was provided by Duke University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Both authors contributed to the study design. W.Z. performed testing, data collection, data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of B.H. Both authors wrote the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wen Zhou.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhou, W., Hare, B. The Early Expression of Blatant Dehumanization in Children and Its Association with Outgroup Negativity. Hum Nat 33, 196–214 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-022-09427-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-022-09427-x

Keywords

Navigation