Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Sep 27;8(10):856.
doi: 10.3390/children8100856.

Selective Attention and Concentration Are Related to Lifestyle in Chilean Schoolchildren

Affiliations

Selective Attention and Concentration Are Related to Lifestyle in Chilean Schoolchildren

Felipe Caamaño-Navarrete et al. Children (Basel). .

Abstract

The objective of this investigation was to determine the association between selective attention and concentration with physical fitness (i.e., cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF), V˙O2max, the standing long jump test (SLJ) and handgrip muscle strength (HGS)), lifestyle parameters (i.e., physical activity (PA) level, screen time (ST), sleep duration and food habits) and anthropometric measures (i.e., body mass index (BMI) and waist circumference (WC)) among Chilean schoolchildren. Two hundred and forty-eight schoolchildren (137 boys, 111 girls, 11.80 ± 1.17 and 11.58 ± 1.09 years, respectively) participated. Selective attention, concentration and lifestyle (PA, ST, sleep duration and Mediterranean diet (MD) adherence) were determined using a standard questionnaire. CRF, SLJ, HGS and anthropometric indicators (BMI and WC) were also measured. Selective attention showed a positive association with MD adherence score (β; 5.012, p = p < 0.05). Concentration was linked inversely to ST (β; -5.498, p = p < 0.05). Likewise, concentration presented a positive association with MD adherence (β; 2.904, p = p < 0.05). In conclusion, children's lifestyles are related to the selective attention and concentration of children; therefore, promoting healthy habits could be a cost-effective strategy in the promotion of cognitive development, as it relates to selective attention and concentration.

Keywords: cognition; dietary patterns; executive function; schoolchildren; screen time.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Study design.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Selective attention (A), concentration (B), total hits (C), omissions (D) and commissions (E) characteristics in schoolchildren participants by ST groups (<2 h/day/≥2 h/day). (†) Daggers denotes significant differences by group at each respective p-value.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Concentration (A) and total hits (B) scores in schoolchildren participants by MD groups (low, moderate and optimal). (†) Daggers denotes significant differences by group at each respective p-value.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Lilly F.R. Creativity and Cognitive Development in Adolescence. Encycl. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2019:1–10. doi: 10.1002/9781119171492.wecad371. - DOI
    1. Zhan Z., Ai J., Ren F., Li L., Chu C.-H., Chang Y.-K. Cardiorespiratory Fitness, Age, and Multiple Aspects of Executive Function Among Preadolescent Children. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01198. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cohen J.F.W., Gorski M.T., Gruber S.A., Kurdziel L.B.F., Rimm E.B. The effect of healthy dietary consumption on executive cognitive functioning in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Br. J. Nutr. 2016;116:989–1000. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516002877. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Sung J., Wickrama K.A.S. Longitudinal relationship between early academic achievement and executive function: Mediating role of approaches to learning. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2018;54:171–183. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.010. - DOI
    1. Montoya M.F., Susperreguy M.I., Dinarte L., Morrison F.J., San Martin E., Rojas-Barahona C.A., Förster C.E. Executive function in Chilean preschool children: Do short-term memory, working memory, and response inhibition contribute differentially to early academic skills? Early Child. Res. Q. 2019;46:187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.009. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources