Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2013 Sep;43(9):2114-20.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1759-1.

Speech preference is associated with autistic-like behavior in 18-months-olds at risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder

Affiliations

Speech preference is associated with autistic-like behavior in 18-months-olds at risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder

Suzanne Curtin et al. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013 Sep.

Abstract

We examined whether infants' preference for speech at 12 months is associated with autistic-like behaviors at 18 months in infants who are at increased risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) because they have an older sibling diagnosed with ASD and in low-risk infants. Only low-risk infants listened significantly longer to speech than to nonspeech at 12 months. In both groups, relative preference for speech correlated positively with general cognitive ability at 12 months. However, in high-risk infants only, preference for speech was associated with autistic-like behavior at 18 months, while in low-risk infants, preference for speech correlated with language abilities. This suggests that in children at risk for ASD an atypical species-specific bias for speech may underlie atypical social development.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Partial regression plot of speech preference index on AOSI total score (centered) adjusting for Mullen ELC score for the SIBS-A group, (r(19) =−.512, p = .018).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Partial regression plot of speech preference index on AOSI total score (centered) adjusting for Mullen ELC score for the SIBS-TD group, r(18) = .084, p = .725.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Behrmann M, Thomas C, Humphreys K. Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:258–264. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001. - PubMed
    1. Bird G, Catmur C, Silani G, Frith C, Frith U. Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage. 2006;31:1614–1624. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.037. - PubMed
    1. Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, McDermott C, Rombough V, Brian J. The autism observational scale for infants: Scale development and reliability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities. 2008;38:731–738. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0440-y. - PubMed
    1. Butterfield EC, Siperstein GN. Influence of contingent auditory stimulation upon non-nutritional suckle. In: Bosma JF, editor. Third symposium on oral sensation and perception: The mouth of the infant. Charles C. Thomas; Springfield, IL: 1970. pp. 313–334.
    1. Chawarska K, Volkmar F, Klin A. Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:178–185. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types