This is the html version of the file http://ndl.ethernet.edu.et/bitstream/123456789/65494/1/29.pdf.pdf#page=618.
Google automatically generates html versions of documents as we crawl the web.
Page 618
CHAPTER 32
Romantic Love
Arthur Aron,
Helen E. Fisher,
Greg Strong
Romantic love appears to be a nearly uni-
versal phenomenon, appearing in every cul-
ture for which data are available (Janowiak
& Fischer, 1992) and in every historical
era (Hatfield & Rapson, 2002). Analogs to
romantic love are found in a wide vari-
ety of higher animal species, and love may
well have played a central role in shap-
ing human evolution (Fisher, 1998, 2004).
Romantic love seems to be a key factor in
quality of life generally, being a source of
both some of the greatest joys and some of
the greatest problems, including depression,
rage, stalking, suicide, and homicide (eg,
Ellis & Malamuth, 2000; for a review, see
Meloy, 1998).
Given the prevalence and importance of
romantic love, it is not surprising that it
has been the subject of both artistic and
scholarly attention from the earliest times.
Among the most significant early scholarly
treatments in Western culture is Plato’s Sym-
posium , a systematic analysis of the nature
of love that continues to be influential today
(eg, Aron & Aron, 1991). There has been a
continuous stream of interest in love since
the classical Greeks, with landmarks that
continue to be influential on contemporary
thought, including Stendhal’s (1822/1927)
book-length essay De l’amour and the exten-
sive discussions of the topic by Freud (eg,
1927) and later writers emerging from that
tradition, such as Theodore Reik (1944) and
Carl Jung (eg, 1959/1925). The 19th and
early 20th century also saw interest in love,
including sociologists studying the family
(eg, Westermarck, 1921), cultural anthro-
pologists (eg, Mead, 1928), and clinical
writers outside of the Freudian tradition
(eg, Grant, 1957).
Scholarly work on romantic love in the
last few decades has been primarily centered
in social and personality psychology, largely
starting with the groundbreaking work of
Donn Byrne (1971) and other influential
work on romantic attraction (eg, Hatfield
[Walster], Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman,
1966) and Berscheid and Hatfield [Walster]’s
(1969) significant distinction between com-
panionate and passionate love. This work
was quickly followed by important contri-
butions of Rubin (1970, 1974) on loving
595
Page 619
596 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
and liking, Dutton and Aron (1974) on the
arousal–attraction effect, and an influential
book on attraction edited by Huston (1974).
The 1980s set the stage for much of the
current thinking on romantic love, includ-
ing the development of lay understandings
of love (Fehr, 1988), the influential exten-
sion of attachment theory to adult love
(Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Shaver & Mikulin-
cer, this volume), Sternberg’s (1986) triangu-
lar theory of love, Tennov’s (1979) descrip-
tive work on intense passionate love, Aron
and Aron’s (1986) self-expansion model of
love, evolutionary psychology approaches
(eg, Buss, 1989), and Hendrick and Hen-
drick’s (1986) adaptation of Lee’s (1977)
model of types of love into a psycho-
metrically solid and widely used multi-
dimensional scale.
These trends from the 1960s through
1980s have all continued and expanded into
the present, with the early 1990s bring-
ing some new strands, such as a stronger
interest in cultural differences (eg, Hat-
field & Rapson, 1996), work on unrecip-
rocated love (Aron, Aron, & Allen, 1998;
Baumeister, Wotman, & Stillwell, 1993), and
love ideals (eg, Fletcher, Simpson, Thomas,
& Giles, 1999; Rusbult, Onizuka, & Lip-
kus, 1993). The major developments in
the late 1990s and early 21st century have
included a new exploration of love as an
emotion (eg, Gonzaga, Keltner, Londahl, &
Smith, 2001) and the dramatic new devel-
opments in the biology of love (eg, Fisher,
1998), notably including most recently the
work on oxytocin and vasopressin in monog-
amous prairie voles (eg, Carter et al.,
1997; Lim, Murphy, & Young, 2004; Young,
Wang, & Insel, 1998), the related work
it has inspired in humans (eg, Gonzaga,
2002), and the recent neuroimaging stud-
ies of romantic love (Aron, Fisher, Mashek,
Strong, Li, & Brown, 2004; Bartels &
Zeki, 2000).
This chapter is an overview of the cur-
rent state of knowledge of romantic love,
noting as appropriate both the sources of the
ideas and the latest thinking and findings. We
conclude with some comments on potential
future directions.
What Is Romantic Love?
In this section, we first review research on
how ordinary people construe love. Then
we turn to how researchers have understood
and measured love, organizing our discus-
sion around the theme of types of love.
How Ordinary People Construe Love
Fehr (1988, 2001) suggested that the long-
standing philosophical controversies over
the meaning of love and the corresponding
diversity of conceptual and operational def-
initions in the scientific literature are due to
the possibility that ordinary people recog-
nize instances of love not by their conform-
ing to some formal definition but rather by
their family resemblance to a prototypical
examplar (just as people seem to recognize
something as a fruit by its similarity to an
apple). Thus, Fehr (1988) adapted Mervis
and Rosch’s (1981) prototype approach to
the topic of love. Specifically, she first had
a group of participants simply list words
that they considered the features of love.
She then took the features listed by more
than one individual and had another sam-
ple rate them for centrality to the concept.
The result was striking agreement across per-
sons in relative centrality of the different fea-
tures, such that some features were central
(eg, caring, intimacy) and others, although
clearly part of the concept, were more
peripheral (eg, butterflies in the stomach,
euphoria). Additional studies demonstrated
that the various prototypical features iden-
tified in this way, and particularly the most
central features, were used by people to rec-
ognize instances of love and that these fea-
tures structured processing and memory for
love-related information.
One line of research emerging from Fehr’s
work has focused on generalizability. Fehr’s
studies were with North American students.
However, replications with other age groups
and in a number of other societies have pro-
duced sets of features of love with clear pro-
totype structures, and in most cases even
the actual content (the particular features
and relative centrality of those features) have
Page 620
romantic love 597
been found to be reasonably common across
age groups and cultures (reviewed in Fehr,
2001). Another line of research emerging
from Fehr’s prototype work focuses on the
latent structure of the prototypical love fea-
tures. Across seven studies, Aron and West-
bay (1996) identified and cross-validated
three latent dimensions of these features,
intimacy (which included mainly features
with the highest centrality ratings), commit-
ment (mainly the next most central items),
and passion (mainly the least central items).
Other approaches to how ordinary peo-
ple understand love have included Shaver,
Schwartz, Kirson, and O’Connor’s (1987)
prototype work in which they found that
love and joy are related or similar, but love
is more personalized toward the object of
affection, whereas joy is more general in
nature. They also noted that people describe
love as a form of social contact that is highly
specific and focused on the love object, a
desire to be near to, to touch, kiss (and
so forth) the loved person. Yet another
approach has been to focus not on proto-
typical features but on prototypical kinds of
love. Thus Fehr and Russell (1991) found that
maternal and friendship love were prototyp-
ical of love in general, but romantic and sex-
ual love were not.
In sum, it appears that people have a com-
mon understanding of what love means in
terms of its resemblance to a set of proto-
type features, that this kind of understanding
is found almost everywhere, although there
are some differences in its content across
cultures; that in North American culture the
central features tend to be related to inti-
macy, the next most central to commitment,
and the more peripheral to passion; and that
romantic love is not the most prototypical
of love in general.
Scientific and Scholarly Delineations:
Types of Love
Scientific and scholarly work on the nature
of love has mainly emphasized identify-
ing and differentiating subspecies or aspects
of love. Most centrally is the distinction
between romantic love (the focus of this
chapter) and more general kinds of love,
such as familial love, compassionate love for
strangers, love of God, or love of country.
As noted, the focus of this chapter is on
romantic love, love in the context of roman-
tic relationships–that is, relationships of the
kind that typically have an explicit actual
or potential sexual component, such as dat-
ing and marital relationships. Aron and Aron
(1991) defined love as “the constellation of
behaviors, cognitions, and emotions associ-
ated with a desire to enter or maintain a
close relationship with a specific other per-
son”(p. 26).
With regard to the relation of roman-
tic love to other relationship constructs,
Rubin (1970) explicitly distinguished lov-
ing from liking, and developed a measure
that included separate scales for each. His
13-item love scale emphasizes dependence,
caring, and exclusiveness and was validated
in part by showing that college dating cou-
ples who scored higher on the scale gazed
longer into their partner’s eyes. His paral-
lel liking scale, on the other hand, empha-
sizes similarity, respect, and positive evalua-
tion. Importantly, the two scales were only
moderately correlated. Indeed, in one study
(Wong, 1989), intensity of unrequited love
was positively correlated with the love scores
and negatively correlated with liking scores.
Rubin’s scale has been widely used, and
other researchers have found the conceptual
distinction between liking and loving to be
very useful (eg, Davis & Todd, 1985; Stern-
berg, 1987).
Turning specifically to romantic love, a
key distinction has been between passion-
ate and companionate love. Berscheid and
Hatfield [Walster](1978) defined the first
as “a state of intense longing for union with
another”(p. 9). They defined companion-
ate love as “the affection we feel for those
with whom our lives are deeply entwined”
(p. 9). Based on their definition of pas-
sionate love, Hatfield and Sprecher (1986)
developed a Passionate Love Scale (PLS).
Example items include “I would rather be
with than with anyone else” and “I melt
when looking deeply into’s eyes.” The
PLS has been used successfully in a wide
Page 621
598 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
variety of studies, including studies that dis-
tinguish what it measures from companion-
ate love (Sprecher & Regan, 1998); most
recently, it was used in an functional mag-
netic resonance imaging (fMRI) study in
which PLS scores correlated with activa-
tion in a region of the caudate associated
with reward (Aron, Fisher, et al., 2004). The
distinction between companionate and pas-
sionate love also maps on to a related dis-
tinction people have been shown to make
between those whom they “love” and the
subset of these with whom they are “in love,”
for whom they also typically report sexual
desire (Myers & Berscheid, 1997).
There has been little explicit attention
devoted to companionate love except as a
conceptual counterpoint to passionate love.
Thus, for example, Masuda (2003) con-
ducted a meta-analysis comparing correla-
tions of passionate and companionate love
with satisfaction involving more than 33
studies by using a variety of measures to rep-
resent each construct (for passionate love
measures, the mean disattenuated correla-
tion with satisfaction was 0.64; for compan-
ionate love measures, the correlation was
either 0.34 for studies using measures of
“friendship love,” or 0.72, for studies using
other measures such as Rubin’s liking or lov-
ing scales and measures of intimacy).
Another influential categorization fo-
cuses on “love styles.” This was originally a
circumplex model of three central and three
secondary love types, based on a combina-
tion of historical conceptions and empiri-
cal analysis of interview reports (Lee, 1977).
However, most research applications have
employed the Hendrick and Hendrick (1986,
2003) measure, which treats Lee’s styles as
six relatively independent dimensions: eros
(romantic, passionate love), ludus (game
playing love), storge (friendship love),
pragma (logical,“shopping-list” love), mania
(possesive, dependent love), and agape (self-
less love).
Yet another influential categorization of
romantic love was developed by Sternberg
(1986), based on his attempt to integrate
the existing psychology and related litera-
tures. Sternberg offers a triangular theory,
which conceptualizes love in terms of inti-
macy, commitment–decision, and passion.
Sternberg treated these three components
as ingredients that in various combinations
define types of love, such as “romantic love”
(the combination of high intimacy, low
commitment, and high passion) or “fatu-
ous love”(high passion, low intimacy, and
high commitment). Sternberg’s three com-
ponents correspond reasonably well with
Aron and Westbay’s (1996) later empiri-
cal identification of latent dimensions of lay
conceptions of love and Sternberg’s concep-
tualization has been independently influen-
tial (eg, Acker & Davis, 1992). However,
research using this conceptualization has
been hampered by the lack of a strong mea-
sure. The questionnaire Sternberg (1997)
developed has been difficult to use because
of problems of discriminant validity among
the scales assessing the three components
(eg, Whitley, 1993).
Hendrick and Hendrick (1989) factor ana-
lyzed many of the measures of love based
on the typologies we have considered (as
well as some others) and identified five latent
dimensions. The first two factors, which
accounted for most of the variance (34 and
14%), Fehr (2001) identified with passionate
and companionate love, respectively. Hen-
drick and Hendrick commented that the
last three factors are “less important but
deserve mention”(p. 791). Factor 3 could
be described as manic or ambivalent love,
Factor 4 as security–closeness, and Factor
5 as a kind of solid, practical, nonerotic,
friendship love.
Before concluding this section, we should
also note that some of the relationship qual-
ities identified as a part or type of love,
also have often been distinguished from love.
One such construct is commitment. Fehr
(1988) demonstrated that lay conceptions of
love and commitment are overlapping but
not identical; Fehr’s (2001) review of stud-
ies using measures of people’s experience
of the two constructs in actual relationships
yielded a similar conclusion. Another such
construct is closeness and intimacy (for a fur-
ther distinction of closeness vs. intimacy, see
Aron & Mashek, 2004). An interesting
Page 622
romantic love 599
theoretical suggestion is that passionate love
is a function of the rate of change in close-
ness (Aron & Aron, 1986) or rate of change in
intimacy (Baumeister & Bratslavsky, 1999).
However, there are no studies to date
directly testing these predictions, and both
closeness and intimacy seem tightly linked to
love (eg, Aron & Westbay [1996] found that
the intimacy dimension was most central to
the prototype of love, and Aron & Fraley
[1999] found that closeness was highly cor-
related with measures of love).
One way to summarize much of this lit-
erature is in terms of passion, intimacy, and
commitment. Passionate love has been dis-
tinguished from the outset from compan-
ionate love; it appears as a distinct factor
in analyses of lay features of love; it is a
strong factor when considering diverse mea-
sures of love; it is described as one of the
three components in Sternberg’s system; and
it is described as eros or mania in Lee’s
and the Hendricks’ system. Other types of
romantic love are less clear-cut. Compan-
ionate love seems to comprise a combina-
tion of commitment and intimacy, is perhaps
deeply linked with relationship satisfaction
more generally, and seems strongly linked
with types of love including friendship love,
practical love, and all-giving (agape) love.
Because most of these other topics have
entire chapters devoted to them elsewhere
in this volume (ie, chapters on commit-
ment and satisfaction), the main focus of
this chapter is on romantic love (the pas-
sionate love aspect), although we continue
to refer briefly to companionate love when
there are unique relevant findings or think-
ing that may not be covered elsewhere in
this volume.
The Biological Basis of Romantic Love
Based on a review of the relevant biological
literature, Fisher (1998) hypothesized that
avian and mammalian species have evolved
three distinct brain systems for courtship,
mating, reproduction, and parenting:(a) the
sex drive, characterized by a craving for sex-
ual gratification;(b) attraction (“favoritism,”
“sexual preference,” or “mate choice”), char-
acterized by focused attention on a preferred
partner, heightened energy, motivation, and
goal-oriented courtship behaviors; and (c)
attachment, characterized by the mainte-
nance of proximity, affiliative gestures, and
expressions of calm when in social contact
with a mating partner and separation anxi-
ety when apart (as well as parental behav-
iors such as territory defense, nest build-
ing, mutual feeding, grooming, and other
parental chores). Each emotion–motivation
system is associated with a different con-
stellation of brain circuits, different behavior
patterns, and different affective states; each
emotion–motivation system varies accord-
ing to the reproductive strategy of each
species; and each emotion–motivation sys-
tem evolved to play a different role in
courtship, mating, reproduction, and par-
enting. The sex drive evolved principally to
motivate individuals to seek sexual union
with a range of partners. Attraction evolved
to motivate individuals to prefer particular
mating partners and focus their courtship
attention on these mates, thereby mak-
ing a mate choice. The system for adult
male–female attachment evolved primarily
to motivate individuals to sustain affilia-
tive connections long enough to complete
species-specific parental duties.
From the perspective of the present chap-
ter on human romantic love, we can equate
Fisher’s “attraction” with passionate love and
Fisher’s “attachment” with companionate
love.(We return later to the issue of the dis-
tinction between the sex drive and roman-
tic love.)
The Biology of Passionate Love
It is well established that many creatures
have mate preferences and make mate
choices. The phenomenon of mate choice
is so common that the ethological litera-
ture regularly uses several terms to describe
it, including “mate choice,”“female choice,”
“mate preference,”“individual preference,”
“favoritism,”“sexual choice,” and “selective
perceptivity.” Fisher (1998; Fisher et al.,
Page 623
600 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
2002) argued that this brain system has a
specific and distinct constellation of neural
correlates; that this system operates in tan-
dem with other neural systems, including
the sex drive and specific sensory circuits
for mate discrimination; that it is expressed
at different times and to different degrees
according to each species’ specific repro-
ductive strategy; and that this brain sys-
tem evolved to enable the chooser to dis-
criminate between courtship displays, prefer
those that advertise superior genes, better
resources, or more parental investment, and
motivate the chooser to focus his or her
courtship attention on and pursue specific
mating partners.
In most species of mammals and birds,
this excitatory state of attraction is brief.
Feelings of attraction last only minutes,
hours, days, or weeks. In humans, Fisher
argued, the neural mechanism for attraction
is more developed, forming the physiologi-
cal basis of what is commonly known as pas-
sionate love, obsessive love, or romantic love.
Ethologists generally lump this system,
attraction, with the sex drive and call
this behavioral–physiological state “procep-
tivity.” There are exceptions. Beach (1976)
made a distinction between the sex drive
and attraction, writing,“The occurrence or
non-occurrence of copulation depends as
much on individual affinities and aversions
as upon the presence or absence of sex
hormones in the female”(p. 131). More-
over,“proceptive and receptive behavior
may depend upon different anatomical and
neurochemical systems in the brain”(p. 131).
Goodall (1986) wrote that “partner prefer-
ences, independent of hormonal influences,
are clearly of major significance for chim-
panzees”(p. 446).
Few scientists have considered the
anatomic and neurochemical mechanisms
that produce mate choice (see Fisher
et al., 2002). However, Beach (1976)
and Liebowitz (1983) proposed that the
neurotransmitters associated with arousal,
dopamine, or norepinephrine (or a combi-
nation of these) may be involved. Fisher
(1998) hypothesized that attraction (roman-
tic love) may be associated with elevated
activity of the brain’s dopamine or nore-
pinephrine and decreased activity of the
brain’s serotonin. These hypotheses are con-
sistent with considerable correlational evi-
dence. Characteristics of intense passionate
love include focused attention, strong moti-
vation, goal-oriented behaviors, heightened
energy, sleeplessness, loss of appetite, feel-
ings of euphoria, obsessive thinking about
the beloved, and heightened attraction dur-
ing adversity in the relationship (eg, Ten-
nov, 1979). Each of these characteristics are
associated with elevated activities of central
dopamine and norepinephrine or decreased
activity of central serotonin in the corre-
sponding brain regions (Flament, Rapoport,
& Bert, 1985; Hollander et al., 1988; Schultz,
2000; Thoren, Asberg, & Bertilsson, 1980;
Wise, 1989; see Fisher 1998). Passionate
attraction takes a variety of graded forms,
however, ranging from romantic love that
is returned to unrequited love. So it is
expected that these gradations of attraction
are associated with different combinations
of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin,
as well as with the activities of many other
neural systems (Fisher, 1998).
Data from animal studies also support the
hypothesis that elevated activities of cen-
tral dopamine play a primary role in attrac-
tion in mammalian species. In rats, block-
ing the activities of dopamine diminishes
specific proceptive behaviors, including hop-
ping and darting (Herbert, 1996). Further,
when a female lab-raised prairie vole is
mated with a male, she forms a distinct pref-
erence for this partner. This preference is
associated with a 50% increase of dopamine
in the nucleus accumbens (Gingrich, Liu,
Cascio, Wang, & Insel, 2000). In fact, when a
dopamine antagonist is injected directly into
the nucleus accumbens, females no longer
prefer this partner and when a female is
injected with a dopamine agonist, she begins
to prefer a conspecific who is present at
the time of infusion, even if the female
has not mated with this male (Aragona,
Yan, Curtis, Stephan, & Wang, 2003; Wang
et al., 1999).
Two recent studies using fMRI lend rel-
atively direct support to the dopamine
Page 624
romantic love 601
hypothesis in humans. fMRI technology
scans the brain to register blood flow changes
in any or all brain regions that are either
increasing or decreasing their metabolic
activities. Bartels and Zeki (2000) scanned
a group of participants who reported being
“truly, deeply, and madly in love”(p. 3829),
and compared brain activation when looking
at the beloved partner versus when looking
at familiar friends. They found a specific con-
stellation of brain activity associated with
looking at the beloved, including activity in
the caudate nucleus. The caudate nucleus is
largely associated with motivation and goal-
oriented behaviors; 80% of receptor sites for
dopamine reside here, and the caudate is a
central part of the brain’s “reward system,”
the system associated with the identification
of, focus on, and motivation to win rewards.
These data suggest that passionate romantic
love is primarily a motivation system associ-
ated with dopamine pathways in the reward
system of the brain.
Aron, Fisher, et al.(2004) conducted a
similar study, but their participants were
more recently and even more intensely in
love than those in the Bartels and Zeki study.
(In the Aron et al. sample, mean time in
love was 7 months and mean PLS score was
8.54 on a 9-point scale; in the Bartels and
Zeki sample, the corresponding means were
29 and 7.55 months). In the Aron et al.
study, comparison of activations when look-
ing at and thinking about a beloved (vs. look-
ing at and thinking about familiar neutral
individual) again yielded significant activa-
tion in the caudate. Indeed, in this study,
the caudate activation was especially strong.
Further, Aron et al. found that this cau-
date activation was significantly correlated
(0.60) with scores on the PLS.(Bartels &
Zeki did not test this correlation.) Most
important, Aron et al. also found signifi-
cant activity in the right ventral tegmental
area, a region primarily associated with the
production and distribution of dopamine to
several other brain regions. These data fur-
ther suggest that dopamine plays a central
role in the focused attention, motivation,
and goal-oriented behaviors associated with
romantic love.
In sum, the considerable data on mate
preference in mammalian (and avian)
species, and the association of this mate
preference with subcortical dopaminergic
pathways in human and animal studies sug-
gest that attraction in mammals (and its
human counterpart, romantic love) is a spe-
cific biobehavioral brain system; that it is
associated with at least one specific neuro-
transmitter, dopamine; and that this brain
system evolved to facilitate a specific repro-
ductive function: mate preference and pur-
suit of this preferred mating partner.
The Biology of Companionate Love
As noted earlier, companionate love overlaps
with intimacy and commitment and general
relationship satisfaction, topics treated else-
where in this volume. Thus, the focus of this
chapter is mainly on passionate love. Never-
theless, we should mention that there has
been some important work on love more
generally, specifically on adult male–female
attachment behaviors in other mammalian
species and fMRI data on maternal love
in humans.
Several brain chemicals have been impli-
cated in male–female bonding, group bond-
ing, and mother–infant bonding in mammals
(see Pedersen, Caldwell, Peterson, Walker, &
Mason, 1992). Recent data indicate that oxy-
tocin and vasopressin are the primary neu-
rohormones associated with monogamous
male–female attachment and monogamous
parenting behaviors in mammals (Carter
et al., 1997; Lim et al., 2004; Young et al.,
1998;). Moreover, the distribution of recep-
tor sites associated with these neurohor-
monal systems in the brain are directed
by specific genes (Lim et al., 2004; Young,
Nilsen, Waymire, MacGregor, & Insel, 1999)
and these systems vary from one species
to the next, contributing to species dif-
ferences in male–female attachment (Lim
et al., 2004).
Recent fMRI studies of humans have also
begun to record the brain regions associated
with maternal love (Bartels & Zeki, 2004;
Swain et al., 2004), and some of these results
suggest that central oxytocin and vasopressin
Page 625
602 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
systems also play a role in mother–infant
attachment (Bartels & Zeki, 2004; Leckman
et al., 2004). Researchers are beginning to
pinpoint some of the neural mechanisms
that most likely contribute to human attach-
ment, specifically male–female companion-
ate love and maternal love.
The Course of Love
Initial Attraction
Other chapters in this volume focus on
attraction (see also Berschied & Reis, 1998)
and courtship (see especially Surra, Gray,
Boettcher, Cottle, & West). Thus, we review
only briefly the extensive literature on
romantic attraction, focusing specifically on
the especially intense attractions commonly
referred to as “falling in love.”
Research over the years has identified sev-
eral factors that lead to general liking, which
also have been found to play a role in specif-
ically romantic attraction. These include
reciprocal liking (discovering that the other
likes the self; eg, Walster & Walster, 1963);
desirability of the other (kindness, intel-
ligence, humor, good looks, social status,
etc.; eg, Buss, 1989); similarity, especially
of attitudes, personality, and demographic
characteristics (eg, Byrne, 1971; Laumann,
Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Rush-
ton, 1989); exposure (eg, Zajonc, 1968);
and social appropriateness, support, and
encouragement from one’s social network
(eg, Sprecher et al., 1994).
In the specific context of falling in love,
reciprocal liking and desirability of the part-
ner appear to be the most influential (Aron,
Dutton, Aron, & Iverson, 1989), even across
cultures (Buss, 1989; Sprecher et al., 1994).
For example, Aron et al.(1989) reported that
in their sample of Canadian college students
who very recently fell in love, approximately
90% of accounts mentioned some indicator
of perceiving the other was attracted to the
self (with eye contact being a particularly
common reported cue) and approximately
78% of accounts mentioned desirable char-
acteristics. They commented that these data
suggest “people are just waiting for an attrac-
tive person to do something they can inter-
pret as liking them”(p. 251).
Among desirable characteristics, across
many cultures, kindness and intelligence
(Buss, 1989) seem to be especially important
for both women and men. Men and women
do seem to vary in their mate preferences,
however. Men are somewhat more likely to
be attracted to women who show visual signs
of youth, health, and beauty; women tend
to be somewhat more attracted to men who
exhibit signs of status and resources (eg,
Buss, 1989; Li, Bailey, & Kenrick, 2002).
Regarding similarity, perceived shared
attitudes plays a highly consistent role across
many experiments (Byrne, 1971), but when
other variables are also free to vary, the
effect sizes are often relatively small (eg,
Newcomb, 1956). Further, much of the
effects may be due to reduced attraction
to perceived dissimilars (Rosenbaum, 1986).
It is also clear that perceived similarity is
much more important than actual similarity
(Berscheid & Reis, 1998). Personality sim-
ilarity seems to play a much smaller role
(Botwin, Buss, & Shackelford, 1997; Caspi
& Herbener, 1993). In general dissimilarity
(“opposites attract”) seems to play little posi-
tive role in attraction, although there is some
evidence that when one believes a relation-
ship with an appropriate other is likely, one
may prefer dissimilars over similars (Aron,
Steele, & Kashdan, 2005).
Exposure or “propinquity” may function
mainly as providing an opportunity. There is
little direct evidence for it playing much of a
direct role in falling in love (Aron et al., 1989;
Sprecher et al., 1994), although the possibil-
ity that platonic friendships are a common
beginning for romantic relationships given
the romantic attractions they often include
(Kaplan & Keys, 1997) may be due to such
an effect.
Social appropriateness and the impact of
social networks has been relatively unex-
plored. Sprecher et al.(1994) found that
social networks play a more important
role in Japanese than in American culture,
perhaps consistent with the former being
more collectivist. There is some evidence in
Page 626
romantic love 603
American culture for a “Romeo and Juliet
Effect” in which romantic love is inversely
correlated with parental approval (Driscoll,
Davis, & Lipetz, 1972); however, most stud-
ies support the more universal pattern of
parental approval being a positive factor
(Sprecher, Felmlee, & Orbuch, 2002).
In addition to these general attraction
variables, Aron et al.(1989) argued that
there are at least three variables that appear
to be specific to falling in love: arousal at
time of meeting the partner (the “arousal–
attraction effect”), readiness for falling in
love, and “specific cues.” The arousal–
attraction effect has been demonstrated in
a series of experiments including the Dut-
ton and Aron (1974)“shaky bridge” study in
which male participants were more attracted
to a good-looking confederate when the par-
ticipant met her on an anxiety-provoking
suspension bridge than when they met her
on a solid, low bridge. Subsequent studies
(see Foster, Witcher, Campbell, & Green,
1998) have demonstrated the generalizabil-
ity of the effect under a great variety of
positive and negative sources of arousal,
as well as supporting at least two mecha-
nisms (reattribution of arousal and eliciting
of a dominant response). One recent study
(Lewandowski & Aron, 2004) showed the
effect generalizes across women and men
and holds even when the partner is not a
confederate.
The main direct support for a readiness
effect comes from the Aron et al.(1989)
study in which it was mentioned with mod-
erate frequency in accounts of falling in love
and the Sprecher et al.(1994) cross-cultural
study in which Russian, Japanese, and US
participants all rated it as being moderately
important for falling in love. Indeed, it seems
reasonable that people are less likely to fall
in love with Person A when they have just
fallen in love with Person B and may be more
likely to fall in love when they have just bro-
ken up with someone.
The role of specific cues was first sug-
gested by Binet (1887), the inventor of the
intelligence test, who noted that individuals
are often strongly attracted to others with
some very specific characteristic (a color of
hair, shape of face, way of walking, etc.), a
theme extended by Grant (1957). Aron et al.
(1989) found a number of accounts of falling
in love that seemed especially well explained
by such a phenomenon; perhaps consistent
with studies showing that people often select
romantic partners similar to their parents
(eg, Aron et al., 1974; Little, Penton-Voak,
& Burt, 2003).
Effects of Falling in Love
Taking a largely qualitative approach, Ten-
nov (1979) studied individuals who reported
intense romantic love. As noted earlier,
such individuals commonly report focused
attention, strong motivation, goal-oriented
behaviors, heightened energy, sleeplessness,
loss of appetite, feelings of euphoria, obses-
sive thinking about the beloved, and height-
ened attraction during adversity in the rela-
tionship, characteristics that correspond well
with those emphasized in Hatfield and
Sprecher’s (1986) PLS described earlier.
Is falling in love a good thing? Based
on the self-expansion model (Aron, Aron,
& Norman, 2001, reviewed below), Aron,
Paris, and Aron (1995) predicted that falling
in love, when reciprocated, would lead to an
enhancement of the self-concept, including
increased identity domain, greater sense of
self-efficacy, and greater self-esteem. They
studied two large samples of mainly first-
and second-year US college students, col-
lecting data every 2 weeks over the first 10
weeks of the fall term. In both studies, at
each testing participants completing a series
of items about what had happened in the
last 2 weeks, among which were items about
whether they had fallen in love. In addition,
in the first study, at each testing, they also
answered an open-ended question “Who are
you today?”; in the second study, they com-
pleted standard self-efficacy and self-esteem
scales. About 25% of participants fell in love
at some point over the 10 weeks. The key
results were that participants who fell in
love showed significant increases in diver-
sity of the self-concept and increased self-
efficacy and self-esteem from the testing ses-
sion before to the testing session after they
Page 627
604 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
fell in love. These changes were significantly
greater than the changes across other testing-
to-testing periods for the participants who
fell in love and also significantly greater than
the average testing-to-testing changes for the
participants who did not fall in love. Fur-
ther, all of these results remained significant
even after statistically controlling for mood
changes associated with falling in love.
Unreciprocated Love
Of course, falling in love need not result
in it being reciprocated. Baumeister et al.
(1993) compared autobiographical accounts
of being rejected and of being the object of
someone’s undesired attraction. They found
that rejection can lead to strong organization
as well as strong disorganization of thoughts,
behaviors, and emotions; both the rejector’s
and rejectee’s behaviors are mostly passive;
and both wish (but don’t necessarily act) for
different behaviors and outcomes from the
other and both usually end up disappointed.
Aron et al.(1998) found that intensity
of unrequited love was predicted by three
factors. The most important was perceived
desirability of the partner and the relation-
ship (eg, high ratings for “How perfect is
this person in your eyes?”); the second most
important was perceived desirability of the
state of being in love, whether reciprocated
or not (eg,“How fulfilling is it to love this
person even though it is unrequited?”); and
the least important (but still significant) was
mistakenly believing at the outset that the
other would reciprocate the love (eg,“Even
though you don’t feel this person loves you
as much as you would like, to what extent
has this person done things that would
make most people think he or she loves
you?”). Aron et al.(1998) also found dif-
ferences by self-reported attachment style.
Secure individuals were least likely to expe-
rience unrequited love; when they did, they
were the group with the strongest associa-
tion with mistaken expectation of recipro-
cation. Avoidant individuals were the next
most likely to experience unrequited love;
when they did, they were the group with
the strongest association with desirability of
the state of being in love. Finally, anxious–
ambivalent individuals were the most likely
to experience unrequited love and were the
group with the strongest association with
desirability of the partner (indeed, it was
extremely strong for them, and intensity for
them was negatively associated with desir-
ability of the state of being in love).
Maintaining Love Over Time
Relationship satisfaction and measures of
companionate love generally show declines
over time after the initial relationship period
(Karney & Bradbury, 1995; Tucker & Aron,
1993). Indeed, one 5-year longitudinal study
of dating couples (Sprecher, 1999) found
that although reports of love declined over
each year, participants at the end of each
year reported that they loved their partner
more. Thus, it is possible that either people
believe that love increases even when it does
not (consistent with Karney & Frye’s [2002]
findings on recall of satisfaction), or perhaps
the meaning of love changes so that what was
considered love last year is now considered
a shallow affection.
In any case, with regard to passionate
love, the general view among love theo-
rists has been that if a romantic relationship
persists over time, passionate love declines
over the first couple years (eg, Huesman,
1980; Sternberg, 1986), and, if things are
going well, companionate love correspond-
ingly increases (eg, Berscheid & Hatfield
[Walster], 1969; Sternberg, 1986), creating
perhaps a “warm afterglow”(Reik, 1944).
Consistent with this view, cross-sectional
data show that passionate love is higher at
marriage than either just before the birth
of a couple’s first child or just before the
last child leaves home, and longitudinal data
show that it is higher before than after mar-
riage (Tucker & Aron, 1993) and from the
first to second year of marriage (Traupmann
& Hatfield, 1981; Utne, 1977). Pineo (1961)
found declines in self-report items of phys-
ical attraction and romantic feeling in 400
married couples from engagement to 20
years later.
Page 628
romantic love 605
Several theories have been offered to
explain the general decline in passionate
love over time. One approach emphasizes
habituation (eg, Aronson & Linder, 1965;
Berger, 1988; Huesmann, 1980). Another
view emphasizes the evolutionary value of
passionate love for initiating and maintaining
the relationship over the early stages, or long
enough to conceive a child (eg, Fisher, 1998,
2004). Yet another perspective emphasizes
that passion arises from the rapid develop-
ment of the relationship. Thus, Aron and
Aron (1986) argued that passion arises from
the intensity of the rapid self-expansion that
occurs in the formation of a relationship as
one comes to include the other in the self;
after the other is largely included, the rate of
expansion inevitably slows down. Baumeis-
ter and Bratslavsky (1999) offered a similar
model, emphasizing that passion is a func-
tion of the rate of increase in intimacy and
that as intimacy plateaus, passion decreases.
Nevertheless, while passionate love (and
satisfaction and love of all kinds) gen-
erally declines over time, the view that
passionate love inevitably declines has not
been demonstrated. It is certainly clear that
many long-term couples experience high
levels of satisfaction (eg, Cuber & Har-
roff, 1965). Indeed, in a 4-year longitudinal
study of newlyweds, Karney and Bradbury
(1997) found that about 10% maintained
or increased their level of satisfaction. Per-
haps more surprising, several cross-sectional
studies have found a small percentage of
individuals in long-term relationships of 20
years or more report very high levels of pas-
sionate love (reviewed in Tucker & Aron,
1993). Preliminary results of an interview
study (Acevedo & Aron, 2005) suggest that
at least some such reports may correspond to
how the relationship is actually being expe-
rienced and not due merely to response bias
or self-deception. Further, Aron, Norman,
Aron, McKenna, and Heyman (2000) were
able to increase reported passionate love (at
least temporarily) in long-term relationship
partners through an experimental task, sug-
gesting there may be natural mechanisms
that permit high levels of passionate love
even in long-term relationships.
How Does Love Work?(Models
of Love Processes)
In this section, we briefly review seven
approaches that have been particularly influ-
ential in specifically focusing on understand-
ing the dynamics of romantic love in general,
and especially with regard to passionate love.
Other important theoretical approaches in
the relationship area, such as interdepen-
dence theory (Kelley & Thibaut, 1979),
have only rarely been applied specifically to
romantic love (eg, Kelley, 1983; Rusbult
et al., 1993)
Cultural Models
As noted at the outset, romantic love has
been observed in every culture in which
observers have reported on relevant topics
and in every era of human history (Fisher,
2004; Hatfield & Rapson, 2002; Jankowiak
& Fischer, 1992). However, the extent to
which it is valued by a culture, the role it
plays in marriage, and the traditional exem-
plars and narratives seem to differ greatly
across cultures (eg, Dion & Dion, 1988,
1996; Hatfield, Martel, & Rapson, in press;
Hatfield & Rapson, 1996; Hendrick & Hen-
drick, 2003). For example, Dion and Dion
focused on individualism and collectivistic
views or attitudes. They argued that individ-
ualistic people may have a difficult time lov-
ing and becoming intimate with each other.
The high divorce rate in the United States
may be due to exaggerated feelings of indi-
vidualism. As another example, Sprecher
et al.(1994) compared love experiences of
college students in the United States, Japan,
and Russia. Across the three cultures, most
participants had been in love at least once,
erotic love was the most common style, most
believed that love should be the basis of mar-
riage, and desirable personality and physi-
cal appearance and reciprocal liking were
most important for falling in love. There
were also differences: Americans had more
secure attachment, were higher on the eros
and storge love styles and on passionate love,
and considered physical appearance and sim-
ilarity more important for falling in love.
Page 629
606 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
Russians scored higher on avoidant attach-
ment, ludus love style and agape style, and
were most willing to marry someone they
didn’t love romantically (41% of Russian
women and 30% of Russian men reported
that); Russians rated familiarity high but per-
sonality and similarity low as reasons they
had fallen in love. Japanese were least likely
to be in love at the time and more likely
to have never been in love; they had more
avoidants, were less romantic, and consid-
ered social standing more important.
Overall, these examples and other stud-
ies of cultural (and subcultural) differences
and similarities (eg, Contreras, Hendrick, &
Hendrick, 1996; Doherty, Hatfield, Thomp-
son, & Choo, 1994; Kim, Hatfield, & Kim,
2004; Levine, Sato, Hashimoto, & Verma,
1995; Simmons, vom Kolke, & Shimizu,
1986) suggest that there is a core element
of passionate love that arises in every cul-
ture and that may even have an evolution-
ary foundation, but how it is enacted may
depend heavily on the cultural context.
Love As Emotion
Many emotion theorists have treated love
as an emotion (eg, Gonzaga et al., 2001)
or even as a basic emotion (Shaver, Mor-
gan, & Wu, 1996; Shaver et al., 1987), not-
ing, for example, that it is typically the first
response given when participants are asked
for an example of an emotion and, partic-
ularly when one focuses on “moments of
love,” it shows many of the features of emo-
tions. On the other hand, Aron and Aron
(1991) argued that although love is highly
emotional, it may be better characterized as
a goal-oriented motivational state and not as
a specific emotion in its own right, given that
it tends to be hard to control, is not asso-
ciated with any specific facial expression,
and is focused on a specific reward. To date,
this latter view is supported by two lines of
research. First, in various studies (Acevedo
& Aron, 2004a; Rousar, 1990) asking partici-
pants to check the emotions one feels or has
felt when experiencing “love”(or “roman-
tic love,”“passionate love,” or a “moment
of passionate love”), many more emotions
are checked than are checked for fear, anger,
sadness, or happiness; in each case, there
were also more opposite valence emotions
checked when rating experiences of love
than experiences of fear and so forth. These
results were predicted based on the idea that
like other goal oriented states, love gener-
ates a variety of specific emotions according
to the extent to which it is satisfied or frus-
trated. The second line of work is the recent
fMRI studies of romantic love (Aron Fisher
et al., 2005; Bartels & Zeki, 2000), which,
as noted earlier, found activation across par-
ticipants primarily in reward-related brain
regions, with greater diversity of response in
emotion-related regions.
At this point it seems clear that passionate
love has a strong motivational component
and functions much like a goal state. Never-
theless, it remains possible that love may also
be a specific emotion or represent a specific
motivational experience. Of course, both a
constellation of emotions and several moti-
vations are clearly involved, and definitions
of what are called emotions versus motiva-
tions are somewhat overlapping.
Love As Sex
Ellen Berscheid (1988) made the influen-
tial comment that passionate love is “about
90% sexual desire unfulfilled.” Clearly, sex-
ual desire plays a significant role in pas-
sionate love. For example, in the lay pro-
totype of love developed by Fehr (1988),
many of the features identified by Aron and
Westbay (1996) as part of the passion fac-
tor are sexual in nature, including sexual
passion, sex appeal, and physical attraction.
Similarly, the PLS Scale, the most widely
used measure of passionate love, includes
items that emphasize sexual desire, includ-
ing “I sense my body responding when
touches me,”“In the presence of, I yearn to
touch, and be touched,” and “Sometimes my
body trembles with excitement at the sight
of”–all items that correlate highly with
the other scale items.
Nevertheless, it does seem possible to dis-
tinguish passionate love from sexual desire.
Conceptually, Aron and Aron (1991) argued
that understandings of passionate love and
sexuality fall on a continuum from love
Page 630
romantic love 607
being the cause of sex to sex being the cause
of love. In terms of evolutionary foundations,
as noted earlier, Fisher (1998) argued that
romantic attraction and the sex drive are
associated with distinct brain systems and
that each evolved to facilitate a different
aspect of courtship, mating, and reproduc-
tion. Several studies also support their being
such a distinction. Gonzaga et al.(2001)
found positive correlations between love and
sexual desire, but also that there are differ-
ent cues and different behavioral responses.
Another relevant line of thinking is Dia-
mond’s (2003) argument that sexual orien-
tation does not completely predict the gen-
der of objects of passionate love and that
individuals sometimes appear to fall in love
with partners of the “wrong” gender with
whom they may have no initial desire to
have sexual contact, even though they show
all the other symptoms of passionate love.
Finally, the two fMRI studies of romantic
love (Aron Fisher et al., 2005; Bartels &
Zeki, 2000) found activations that only min-
imally overlapped with activations that have
been found in studies of sexual arousal (eg,
Arnow et al., 2002; Karama et al., 2002).
In sum, sexuality almost surely plays an
important role in passionate love, but it
is also conceptually and empirically distin-
guishable from it and cannot fully explain
its functioning.
Love As Attachment
Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969; Shaver
& Mikulincer, this volume) has been among
the most influential approaches to under-
standing romantic love and is the primary
approach that emphasizes individual differ-
ences. The theory posits that love develops
out of three behavioral systems that evolved
to promote development and survival of
infants in humans and perhaps other pri-
mates or even other species (Shaver, Hazan,
& Bradshaw, 1988). These systems include
attachment, caregiving, and sexuality. In
human adults, according to this model, pas-
sionate love is a combination of the desire
for attachment and sexuality.(Companion-
ate love develops out of these systems plus
the caregiving system.) Further, this model
emphasizes that early experience with care-
givers (whether they serve as a reliable
secure base for exploration and safe haven
under threat) strongly shapes individual dif-
ferences in adult love experiences. Thus, for
example, those who have had inconsistent
caregiving (those high on the anxious attach-
ment or preoccupied dimension) are much
more likely to experience intense passionate
love and more likely to experience intense
unrequited love, whereas those who expe-
rienced a consistent lack of security (those
high on the avoidance dimension) are espe-
cially unlikely to experience passionate love
in adulthood (Aron et al., 1998; Hendrick
& Hendrick, 1989). Some preliminary evi-
dence even suggests that the brain systems
engaged by passionate love may be moder-
ated by individual differences in attachment
style (Aron, Fisher et al., 2004).
Love as a Story
Sternberg (1998) suggested that loving rela-
tionships can be described accurately by the
people involved through narrative autobi-
ographies, often suggesting culturally proto-
typical “stories.” For example, the story of a
couple locked in constant struggle is com-
mon, as is the story of couples growing to
love each other over time. This approach
seems promising given the general tendency
for people to organize their world in narra-
tive form and there has been some prelimi-
nary research support for the model (Stern-
berg, Hojjat, & Barnes, 2001).
Evolutionary Approaches
Because courtship and mate choice are cen-
tral aspects of reproduction in higher ani-
mals, it seems plausible that the experiences,
behaviors, and neural underpinnings of pas-
sionate love might be strongly shaped by
evolution. Thus, as noted in the section on
the biology of romantic love, Fisher (1998)
proposed that the brain system for roman-
tic attraction evolved to motivate individuals
to select among potential mating partners,
prefer particular conspecifics, and focus their
courtship attention on these favored individ-
uals, thereby conserving precious courtship
and mating time and energy.
Page 631
608 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
As also noted earlier, another important
line of evolutionary thinking, largely based
on parental investment theory (Trivers,
1972), has emphasized gender differences
in what features are desirable in a mate
and in the basis for jealousy (eg, Buss
& Schmitt, 1993). There have also been
some approaches to the evolutionary basis
of experience and behavior in romantic love
arguing that the mating system exploits an
evolved bonding module between infants
and parent (Hazan & Diamond, 2000; Miller
& Fishkin, 1997).
Self-Expansion Model
Aron and Aron’s (1986) self-expansion
model posits (a) a primary human motiva-
tion to expand one’s self in terms of poten-
tial to attain desired goals and (b) that a
main way that people seek to expand their
self is in terms of “including others in the
self” through close relationships so that the
other’s resources, perspectives, and identi-
ties are treated to some extent as one’s
own. Both principles have received consider-
able research support (for reviews, see Aron
et al., 2001; Aron, Mashek, & Aron, 2004).
In terms of romantic love, Aron et al.(2000)
argued that the exhilaration and intense
focused attention of passionate love arises
from the rapid rate of including the other in
the self often associated with forming a new
romantic relationship. We have cited sev-
eral relevant studies throughout this chapter.
Companionate love, they argued, arises from
the ongoing expansion offered by the part-
ner and the potential for loss to the self of los-
ing the partner.(For example, Lewandowski,
Aron, Bassis, & Kunak,[2005] found that
the degree of negative impact on the self-
concept following relationship dissolution
was predicted by degree of ongoing self-
expansion that had been provided by the dis-
solved relationship.)
Summary and Conclusions
Romantic love is a nearly universal phe-
nomenon that has been the subject of schol-
arly interest for centuries but has only in the
last half century been a topic of systematic
scientific study. What has been learned from
this study is that romantic love is understood
by ordinary people in terms of its resem-
blance to a standard prototype and is best
understood by researchers for purposes of
systematic analysis in terms of various types
of love, most centrally in terms of a distinc-
tion between passionate and companionate
love. There has been considerable recent
progress in identifying the biological under-
pinnings of romantic love, including support
from animal data and human neuroimaging
studies for passionate love being linked with
dopamine-based reward processes, whereas
companionate love seems linked with bond-
ing more generally and perhaps specifically
with central oxytocin and vasopressin sys-
tems. The course of romantic love has been
well delineated in terms of predictors of ini-
tial romantic attraction and diverse stud-
ies providing insights and suggested direc-
tions for future research regarding the effects
of falling in love on the self, the processes
and motivations associated with unrequited
love, and the course of passionate love over
time including potential moderators of that
course. Finally, there are now at least seven
major approaches to understanding roman-
tic love that have served as the basis for
much of the research on the topic. These
approaches include cultural models, emo-
tion models, attachment theory, love as sex,
evolutionary theory, love as a story, and the
self-expansion model.
We hope that this review has conveyed
our view that the study of romantic love
is both important and a thriving scientific
endeavor, offering both a solid foundation
and vast opportunities for significant fu-
ture work.
References
Acevedo, B., & Aron, A.(2004a, July). On the
emotional categorization of love and beyond .
Paper presented at the International Associ-
ation for Relationship Research Conference,
Madison, WI.
Page 632
romantic love 609
Acevedo, B., & Aron, A.(2005, June). Intense
romantic love in longterm relationships. Paper
presented at the Positive Psychology Summer
Institute, Philadelphia, PA.
Acker, M., & Davis, MH (1992). Intimacy,
passion, and commitment in adult romantic
relationships: A test of the triangular theory
of love. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 9 , 21–50.
Aragona, BJ, Yan, L., Curtis, JT, Stephan, FK,
& Wang, Z.(2003). A critical role for nucleus
accumbens dopamine in partner-preference
formation in male prairie voles. Journal of Neu-
roscience, 23 , 3483–3490.
Arnow, BA, Desmond, JE, Banner, LL,
Glover, GH, Solomon, A., Polan, ML, et al.
(2002). Brain activation and sexual arousal in
healthy, heterosexual males. Brain, 125 , 1014–
1023.
Aron, A., & Aron, EN (1986). Love and the
expansion of self: Understanding attraction and
satisfaction . New York: Hemisphere.
Aron, A., & Aron, EN (1991). Love and sexual-
ity. In K. McKinney & S. Sprecher (Eds.), Sexu-
ality in close relationships (pp. 25–48). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Aron, A., Aron, EN, & Allen, J.(1998). Motiva-
tions for unreciprocated love. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 , 787–796.
Aron, A., Aron, EN, & Norman, C.(2001). The
self expansion model of motivation and cog-
nition in close relationships and beyond. In G.
Fletcher & M. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook
in social psychology, Volume 2: Interpersonal pro-
cesses (pp. 478–501). Oxford: Blackwell.
Aron, A., Dutton, DG, Aron, EN, & Iver-
son, A.(1989). Experiences of falling in love.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6 ,
243–257.
Aron, A., et al.(1974). Relationships with
opposite-sexed parents and mate choice.
Human Relations, 27 , 17–24.
Aron, A., Fisher, HE, Mashek, D., Strong,
G., Shaver, P., Mikulincer, M., et al.(2004,
August). Individual differences in attachment
anxiety and attachment avoidance: An fMRI
study . Paper presented at the Conference on
Biological Basis of Personality and Individual
Differences, Stony Brook, NY.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D., Strong, G.,
Li, H., & Brown, L.(2005). Neural systems in
intense romantic attraction: An fMRI study Jour-
nal of Neurophysiology , 94 , 327–337.
Aron, A., & Fraley, B.(1999). Relationship close-
ness as including other in the self: Cognitive
underpinnings and measures. Social Cognition,
17 , 140–160.
Aron, A., & Mashek, D.(2004). Conclusion. In
D. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of close-
ness and intimacy (pp. 415–428). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Aron, A., Mashek, D., & Aron, EN (2004).
Closeness, intimacy, and including other in the
self. In D. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook
of closeness and intimacy (pp. 27–41). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Aron, A., Norman, CC, Aron, EN, McKenna,
C., & Heyman, R.(2000). Couples’ shared par-
ticipation in novel and arousing activities and
experienced relationship quality. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 78 , 273–283.
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, EN (1995). Falling
in love: Prospective studies of self-concept
change. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 69 , 1102–1112.
Aron, A., Steele, JL, & Kashdan, T.(2005).
When do opposites attract? Three studies from
a self-expansion model perspective . Manuscript
under review.
Aron, A., & Westbay, L.(1996). Dimensions of
the prototype of love. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 70 , 535–551.
Aronson, E., & Linder, D.(1965). Gain and loss of
esteem as determinants of interpersonal attrac-
tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
1 , 156–171.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S.(2000). The neural basis of
romantic love. NeuroReport, 11 , 1–6.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S.(2004) The neural corre-
lates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroim-
age, 21 , 1155–1166.
Baumeister, RF, & Bratslavsky, E.(1999). Pas-
sion, intimacy, and time: Passionate love as a
function of change in intimacy. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 3 , 49–67.
Baumeister, RF, Wotman, SR, & Stillwell,
AM (1993). Unrequited love: On heart-
break, anger, guilt, scriptlessness, and humili-
ation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 64 , 377–394.
Beach, FA (1976). Sexual attractivity, procep-
tivity, and receptivity in female mammals. Hor-
mones and Behavior, 7 , 105–138.
Berger, CR (1988). Uncertainty and informa-
tion exchange in developing relationships. In S.
Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships:
Page 633
610 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
Theory, research and interventions (pp. 367–
389). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Berscheid, E.(1988). Some comments on love’s
anatomy: Or, whatever happened to old-
fashioned lust? In RJ Sternberg & ML
Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 359–
371). New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Berscheid, E., & Hatfield [Walster], EH (l969).
Interpersonal attraction . New York: Addison-
Wesley.
Berscheid, E., & Hatfield [Walster], EH (1978).
Interpersonal attraction (2nd ed.). Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Berscheid, E., & Reis, HT (1998). Attraction and
close relationships. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, & G.
Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(4th ed., pp. 193–281). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Binet, A.(1887). Le fetischisme dans l’amour.
Revue philosophique, 24 , 260.
Botwin, MD, Buss, DM, & Shackelford, TK
(1997). Personality and mate preferences: Five
factors in mate selection and satisfaction. Jour-
nal of Personality, 65 , 107–136.
Bowlby, J.(1969). Attachment and loss, Volume
1: Attachment . London: Hogarth Press and the
Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Buss DM (1989). Sex differences in human mate
preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in
37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 ,
1–49.
Buss, DM, & Schmitt, DP (1993). Sexual
strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective
on human mating. Psychological Review, 100 ,
204–232.
Byrne, D.(1971). The attraction paradigm . New
York: Academic Press.
Carter, CS, DeVries, AC, Taymans, SE,
Roberts, RL, Williams, JR, & Getz, LL
(1997). Peptides, steroids, and pair bonding. In
CS Carter, II Lederhendler, & B. Kirkpatrick
(Eds.), The integrative neurobiology of affiliation
(Vol. 807, pp. 260–272). Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences .
Caspi, A., & Herbener, ES (1993). Pheno-
typic convergence and marital assortment:
Longitudinal evidence. Social Biology, 40 ,
48–59.
Contreras, R., Hendrick, SS, & Hendrick, C.
(1996). Perspectives on marital love and satis-
faction in Mexican American and Anglo cou-
ples. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74 ,
408–415.
Cuber, JF, & Harroff, P.(1965). The significant
Americans: A study of sexual behavior among the
affluent . New York: Appleton-Century.
Curtis, JT, & Wang, Z.(2003). Forebrain c-fos
expression under conditions conducive to pair
bonding in female prairie voles ( Microtus ochro-
gaster ). Physiology and Behavior, 80 , 95–101.
Davis, KE, & Todd, MJ (1985). Assess-
ing friendship: Prototypes, paradigm cases
and relationship description. In S. Duck &
D. Perlman (Eds.), Understanding personal
relationships: An interdisciplinary approach
(pp. 17–38). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Diamond, LM (2003). What does sexual ori-
entation orient? A biobehavioral model distin-
guishing romantic love and sexual desire. Psy-
chological Review, 110 , 173–192.
Dion, KL, & Dion, KK (1988). Romantic love:
Individual and cultural perspectives. In RJ
Sternberg & ML Barnes (Eds.), The psychol-
ogy of love (pp. 264–289). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Dion, KK, & Dion, KL (1996). Cultural per-
spectives on romantic love. Personal Relation-
ships, 3 , 5–18.
Doherty, RW, Hatfield, E., Thompson, K., &
Choo, P.(1994). Cultural and ethnic influences
on love and attachment. Personal Relationships,
1 , 391–398.
Driscoll, R., Davis, KE, & Lipetz, ME (1972).
Parental interference and romantic love: The
Romeo and Juliet effect. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 24 , 1–10.
Dutton, DG, & Aron, A.(1974). Some evidence
for heightened sexual attraction under condi-
tions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 30 , 510–517.
Ellis, B., & Malamuth, NM (2000). Love and
anger in romantic relationships: A discrete sys-
tems model. Journal of Personality, 68 , 3525–
556.
Fehr, B.(1988). Prototype analysis of the concepts
of love and commitment. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 55 , 557–579.
Fehr, B.(2001). The status of theory and research
on love and commitment. In G. Fletcher
& Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook in social
psychology, Volume 2: Interpersonal processes
(pp. 331–336). Oxford: Blackwell.
Fehr, B., & Russell, JA (1991). The concept of
love viewed from a prototypical perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 ,
425–438.
Page 634
romantic love 611
Fisher, HE (1998). Lust, attraction, and attach-
ment in mammalian reproduction. Human
Nature, 9 , 23–52.
Fisher, HE (2004). Why we love: The nature and
chemistry of romantic love . New York: Holt.
Fisher, HE, Aron, A., Mashek, D., Li, H.,
Strong, G., & Brown, LL (2002). The neural
mechanisms of mate choice. Neuroendocrinol-
ogy Letters [Special Issue], 23 (Suppl. 4), 92–
97.
Flament, MF, Rapoport, JL, & Bert, CL
(1985). Clomipramine treatment of childhood
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A double–blind
controlled study. Archives of General Psychia-
try, 42 , 977–986.
Fletcher, GJO, Simpson, JA, Thomas, G.,
& Giles, L.(1999). Ideals in intimate relation-
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 76 , 72–89.
Foster, CA, Witcher, BS, Campbell, WK,
& Green, JD (1998). Arousal and attraction:
Evidence for automatic and controlled pro-
cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 74 , 86–101.
Freud, S.(1927). Some psychological conse-
quences of anatomical distinction between the
sexes. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis,
8 , 133–142.
Gingrich, B., Liu, Y., Cascio, C., Wang, Z., &
Insel, TR (2000). D2 receptors in the nucleus
accumbens are important for social attachment
in female prairie voles ( Microtus ochrogaster ).
Behavioral Neuroscience, 114 , 173–183.
Gonzaga, GC (2002). Distinctions between sex-
ual desire and love in narrative report, non-
verbal expression and physiology. Dissertation
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences
& Engineering, 63 (2-B), 1087.
Gonzaga, GC, Keltner, D., Londahl, EA, &
Smith, MD (2001). Love and the commit-
ment problem in romantic relations and friend-
ship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 81 , 247–262.
Goodall, J.(1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Pat-
terns of behavior . Cambridge MA: The Belknap
Press of Harvard University Press.
Grant, VW (1957). The psychology of sexual
emotions; The basis of selective attraction . New
York: Longmans, Green.
Hatfield, E., Martel, L., & Rapson, RL (in press).
Love and hate. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen
(Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology . New
York: Guilford Press.
Hatfield, E., & Rapson, RL (1996). Love and sex:
Cross-cultural perspectives . Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Hatfield, E., & Rapson, RL (2002). Passionate
love and sexual desire. In HT Reis, MA
Fitzpatrick, & AL Vangelisti (Eds.), Stability
and change in relationships (pp. 306–324). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S.(1986). Measuring
passionate love in intimate relations. Journal of
Adolescelnce, 9 , 383–410.
Hazan, C., & Diamond, LM (2000). The place
of attachment in human mating. Review of Gen-
eral Psychology, 4 , 186–204.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P.(1987). Romantic love
conceptualized as an attachment process. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 , 511–
524.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, SS (1986). A theory
and method of love. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 50 , 392–402.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, SS (1989). Research
on love: Does it measure up? Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 56 , 784–794.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, SS (2003). Romantic
love: Measuring cupid’s arrow. In SJ Lopez
& CR Snyder (Eds.), Positive psychological
assessment: A handbook of models and mea-
sures (pp. 235–249). Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association.
Herbert, J.(1996). Sexuality, stress and the chem-
ical architecture of the brain. Annual Review of
Sex Research, 7 , 1–44.
Hollander, E., Fay, M., Cohen, B., Campeas,
R., Gorman, JM, & Liebowitz, MR
(1988). Serotonergic and noradrenergic sensi-
tivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Behav-
ioral findings. American Journal of Psychiatry,
145 , 1015–1017.
Huesmann, L.(1980). Toward a predictive model
of romantic behavior. In K. Pope (Ed.), On love
and loving (pp. 152–171). San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Huston, T.(Ed.).(1974). Foundations of interper-
sonal attraction . New York: Academic Press.
Jankowiak, WR, & Fischer, EF (1992). A cross-
cultural perspective on romantic love. Ethnol-
ogy, 31 , 149.
Jung, CG (1959). Marriage as a psychological
relationship. In VS DeLaszlo (Ed.), The basic
writings of CG Jung (RFC Hull, Trans.,
pp. 531–544). New York: Modern Library.
(Original work published 1925.)
Page 635
612 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
Kaplan, DL, & Keys, CB (1997). Sex and rela-
tionship variables as predictors of sexual attrac-
tion in cross-sex platonic friendships between
young heterosexual adults. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 14 , 191–206.
Karama, S., Lecours, AR, Leroux, JM, Bour-
gouin, P., Beaudoin, G., Joubert, S., & Beau-
regard, M.(2002). Areas of brain activation in
males and females during viewing of erotic film
excerpts. Human Brain Mapping, 16 , 1–13.
Karney, BR, & Bradbury, TN (1995). The lon-
gitudinal course of marital quality and stability:
A review of theory, methods, and research. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 118 , 3–34.
Karney, BR, & Bradbury, TN (1997). Neuroti-
cism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of
marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 72 , 1075–1092.
Karney, BR, & Frye, NE (2002).“But
we’ve been getting better lately”: Comparing
prospective and retrospective views of relation-
ship development. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 82 , 222–238.
Kelley, HH (1983). Love and commitment. In
HH Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, JH
Harvey, TL Huston, G. Levinger, et al.(Eds.),
Close relationships (pp. 265–314). New York:
Freeman.
Kelley, HH & Thibaut, JW (1979). Interper-
sonal relations: A theory of interdependence . New
York: Wiley-Interscience.
Kim, J., Hatfield, E., & Kim, J,(2004). Love
types and subjective well-being: A cross cul-
tural study. Social Behavior and Personality, 32 ,
173–182.
Laumann, EO, Gagnon, JH, Michael, RT,
& Michaels, S.(1994). The social organization of
sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States .
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leckman, JF, Feldman, R., Swain, JE, Eicher,
V., Thompson, N., & Mayes, LC (2004). Pri-
mary parental preoccupation: Circuits, genes
and the crucial role of the environment. Jour-
nal of Neural Transmission , 111 , 753–771.
Lee, JA (1977). A typology of styles of lov-
ing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
3 , 173–182.
Levine, R., Sato, S., Hashimoto, T., & Verma, J.
(1995). Love and marriage in eleven cultures.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26 , 554–
571.
Lewandowski, GW, & Aron, AP (2004). Dis-
tinguishing arousal from novelty and challenge
in initial romantic attraction. Social Behavior
and Personality, 32 , 361–372.
Lewandowski, GW, Aron, AP, Bassis, S.,
& Kunak, J.(2005). Losing a self-expanding
relationship: Implications for the self-concept .
Manuscript under review.
Li, NP, Bailey, JM, & Kenrick, DT
(2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate
preferences: Testing the tradeoffs. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 82 , 947–955.
Liebowitz, MR (1983). The chemistry of love .
Boston: Little, Brown.
Lim, MM, Murphy, AZ, & Young, LJ (2004).
Ventral striatopallidal oxytocin and vasopressin
V1a receptors in the monogamous prairie vole
( Microtus ochrogaster ). Journal of Comparative
Neurology, 468 , 555–570.
Little, AC, Penton-Voak, IS, & Burt, DM
(2003). Investigating an imprinting-like phe-
nomenon in humans: Partners and opposite-sex
parents have similar hair and eye colour. Evo-
lution and Human Behavior, 24 , 43–51.
Masuda, M.(2003). Meta-analyses of love scales:
Do various love scales measure the same psy-
chological constructs? Japanese Psychological
Research, 45 , 25–37.
Mead, M.(1928). Coming of age in Samoa: A psy-
chological study of primitive youth for Western
civilization . New York: Morrow.
Meloy, JR (1998). The psychology of stalking:
Clinical and forensic perspectives . New York:
Academic Press.
Mervis, CB, & Rosch, E.(1981). Categorization
of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology,
32 , 89–115.
Meyers, SA, & Berscheid, E.(1997). The lan-
guage of love: The difference a preposition
makes. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 23 , 347–362.
Miller, LC, & Fishkin, SA (1997). On the
dynamics of human bonding and reproductive
success: Seeking windows on the adapted-for
human environmental interface. In J. Simpson
& DT Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psy-
chology (pp. 197–235). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Newcomb, TM (1956). The prediction of inter-
personal attraction. American Psychologist, 11 ,
575–586.
Pedersen, CA, Caldwell, JD, Peterson, G.,
Walker, CH, & Mason, GA (1992) Oxy-
tocin activation of maternal behavior in the rat.
Annals of the New York Academy Science, 652 ,
58–69.
Page 636
romantic love 613
Pineo, PC (1961). Disenchantment in the later
years of marriage. Marriage and Family Living,
23 , 3–11.
Reik, T.(1944). A psychologist looks at love . New
York: Farrar & Reinhart.
Rosenbaum, ME (1986). The repulsion
hypothesis: On the nondevelopment of rela-
tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 51 , 1156–1166.
Rousar, EE (1990). Valuing’s role in roman-
tic love . Unpublished dissertation, California
Graduate School of Family Psychology, San
Rafel, CA.
Rubin, Z.(1970). Measurement of romantic love.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16 ,
265–273.
Rubin, Z.(1974). From liking to loving: Pat-
terns of attraction in dating relationships. In
TL Huston (Ed.), Foundations of interpersonal
attraction (pp. 383–402). New York: Academic
Press.
Rusbult, CE, Onizuka, RK, & Lipkus, I.
(1993). What do we really want? Mental mod-
els of ideal romantic involvement explored
through multidimensional scaling. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 29 , 493–527.
Rushton, JP (1989). Epigenesis and social prefer-
ence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 , 31–32.
Schultz, W.(2000). Multiple reward signals in
the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 1 , 199–
207.
Shaver, PR, Hazan, C., & Bradshaw, D.(1988).
Love as attachment: The integration of three
behavioral systems. In RJ Sternberg & ML
Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68–
99). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Shaver, PR, Morgan, HJ, & Wu, S.(1996). Is
love a “basic” emotion? Personal Relationships,
3 , 81–96.
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor,
C.(1987). Emotion knowledge: Further explo-
ration of a prototype approach. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52 , 1061–
1086.
Simmons, CH, vom Kolke, A., & Shimizu, H.
(1986). Attitudes toward romantic love among
American, German, and Japanese students.
Journal of Social Psychology, 126 , 327–336.
Sprecher, S.(1999).“I love you more today than
yesterday”: Romantic partners’ perceptions of
changes in love and related affect over time.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 ,
46–53.
Sprecher, S., Aron, A., Hatfield, E., Cortese, A.,
Potapova, E., & Levitskaya, A.(1994). Love:
American style, Russian style and Japanese
style. Personal Relationships, 1 , 349–369.
Sprecher, S., Felmlee, D., & Orbuch, TL (2002).
Social networks and change in personal rela-
tionships. In AL Vangelisti, HT Reis, &
MA Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Stability and change
in relationships (pp. 257–284). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Sprecher, S., & Regan, PC (1998). Passionate
and companionate love in courting and young
married couples. Sociological Inquiry, 68 , 163–
185.
Stendhal (pseudonym for Beyle, MH)(1927).
On love (VB Holland, Trans). New York: Boni
& Liveright.(Original work published 1822.)
Sternberg, RJ (1986). A triangular theory of
love. Psychological Review, 93 , 119–135.
Sternberg, RJ (1987). Liking versus loving: A
comparative evaluation of theories. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 102 , 331–345.
Sternberg, RJ (1997). Construct validation of a
triangular love scale. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 27 , 313–335.
Sternberg, RJ (1998). Love is a story: A new the-
ory of relationships . London: Oxford University
Press.
Sternberg, RJ, Hojjat, M., & Barnes, ML
(2001). Empirical tests of aspects of a theory
of love as a story. European Journal of Personal-
ity, 15 , 199–218.
Swain, JE, Leckman, JF, Mayes, LC, Feld-
man, R., Constable, RT, & Schultz, RT
(2004). Neuural substrates and psychology of
human parent-infant attachment in the early
postpartum. Biological Psychiatry, 55 , 1535.
Tennov, D.(1979). Love and limerence: The expe-
rience of being in love . New York: Stein & Day.
Thoren, P., Asberg, M., & Bertilsson, L.(1980).
Clomipramine treatment of obsessive disorder:
Biochemical and clinical aspects. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 37 , 1289–1294.
Traupmann, J., & Hatfield, E.(1981). Love and
its effect on mental and physical health. In J.
March, S. Kiesler, R. Fogel, E. Hatfield, & E.
Shanas (Eds.), Aging: Stability and change in
the family (pp. 253–274). New York: Academic
Press.
Trivers, RL (1972). Parental investment and sex-
ual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selec-
tion and the descent of man (pp. 1871–1971).
Chicago: Aldine.
Page 637
614 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
Tucker, P., & Aron, A.(1993). Passionate love and
marital satisfaction at key transition points in
the family life cycle. Journal of Social and Clin-
ical Psychology, 12 , 135–147.
Utne, M.(1977). Equity and intimate relations: A
test of theory in marital interaction . Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin,
Madison.
Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., &
Rottman, L.(1966). The importance of phys-
ical attractiveness in dating behavior. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 ,
508–516.
Walster, E., & Walster, GW (1963). Effect of
expecting to be liked on choice of associates.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 ,
402–404.
Wang, Z, Yu, G., Cascio, C., Liu, Y., Gingrich, B.,
& Insel, TR (1999). Dopamine D2 receptor-
mediated regulation of partner preferences in
female prairie voles ( Microtus ochrogaster ): A
mechanism for pair bonding? Behavioral Neu-
roscience, 113 , 602–611.
Westermarck, E.(1921). The history of human mar-
riage (5th ed.). London: Macmillan.
Whitley, BE (1993). Reliability and aspects of
the construct validity of Sternberg’s Triangular
Love Scale. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 10 , 475–480.
Wise, RA (1989). Brain dopamine and reward.
Annual Review of Psychology, 40 , 191–225.
Wong, PTP (1989, May). Theory and mea-
surement of unrequited love . Paper presented
at the Iowa Conference on Personal Relation-
ships, Iowa City, IA.
Young, LJ, Nilsen, R., Waymire, KG, MacGre-
gor, GR, & Insel, TR (1999). Increased affil-
iative response to vasopresin in mice express-
ing the V1a receptor from a monogamous vile.
Nature, 400 , 766–768.
Young LJ, Wang, Z., & Insel, TR (1998).
Neuroendocrine bases of monogamy. Trends in
Neuroscience, 21 , 71–75.
Zajonc, RB (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere
exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology Monograph Supplement, 9 , 1–27.
Page 638
CHAPTER 33
Commitment
Caryl E. Rusbult
Michael K. Coolsen
Jeffrey L. Kirchner
Jennifer A. Clarke
The past 3 decades have witnessed dramatic
growth in relationships science. Much of this
work has sought to identify the determinants
and consequences of positive affect–attrac-
tion, satisfaction, or love. For example, the
goal of many studies is to explain the causes
of attraction or love; measures of satisfaction
frequently are employed as indices of cou-
ple well-being (for reviews, see Berscheid &
Regan, 2005; Berscheid & Reis, 1998). The
implicit or explicit assumption of this work
is that if partners love each other and feel
happy with their relationship, they should
be more likely to remain involved with one
another. In many respects, this point of view
makes good sense: All things considered, it
is easier to stick with a happy relationship
than a miserable one.
Unfortunately, this conventional focus
on the study of affective reactions fails to
address three key questions: First, why do
some relationships persist despite dissatis-
faction–for example, why do unhappy part-
ners sometimes remain together due to iner-
tia or “for the sake of the children”? Second,
why do some satisfying relationships end–
why do people sometimes abandon rela-
tively happy relationships to pursue desir-
able alternative partners? Third, how can we
account for persistence in the face of ordi-
nary fluctuations in affect? Given that satis-
faction ebbs and flows even in the most grat-
ifying involvements, and given that tempting
alternatives threaten even the most smitten
partners, why do some relationships endure
and thrive whereas others do not?
Over the course of the past 3 decades,
questions such as these have inspired some
social scientists to dedicate themselves to the
study of commitment. Scientists working in
this tradition believe that if we are to com-
prehend fully phenomena such as benev-
olent versus malevolent behavior, positive
versus negative motivation, and tenacious
persistence versus severance, understanding
commitment may be as important as–per-
haps more important than–understand-
ing positive affect. The goal of this chap-
ter is to review work in this tradition. We
begin by describing several formal models of
the causes of commitment. Next, we review
empirical work that is relevant to assessing
the validity of these models, discussing crit-
ical research findings and their implications
615
Page 639
616 the cambridge handbook of personal relationships
for each model. Then we describe several
important commitment processes, examin-
ing the generalizability of commitment phe-
nomena, the role of commitment in inducing
prosocial maintenance behaviors, the asso-
ciation of commitment with trust, and the
phenomenon of deteriorating commitment.
The chapter concludes with a review of con-
temporary trends in the commitment litera-
ture and suggestions for future research.
Formal Models of Commitment
dedicating unwarranted time or effort to an
activity, increasing commitment to a los-
ing enterprise, entrapment in escalating con-
flicts, and the manner in which investments,
side bets, and sunk costs may induce perse-
verance at a line of action (eg, Becker, 1960;
Blau, 1967; Brockner, Shaw, & Rubin, 1979;
Kiesler, 1971; Staw, 1976; Teger, 1980; Trop-
per, 1972). Thus, at the time during which
Levinger, Rusbult, and Johnson advanced
their theories of commitment processes, the
theme of unjustified persistence was promi-
nent throughout the social sciences. 1