Introduction

Almost 100 million disability-adjusted life-years were attributable to alcohol use around the world in 2016 [1]. In 2015, there were an estimated 63.5 million cases of alcohol use disorder (AUD) globally [2]. While psychosocial [3] and pharmaceutical [4] treatment options exist for this disorder, outcomes are moderate at best [5, 6], signifying the need for improved treatment options. The dopamine system has been implicated in AUD with the dopamine D2 receptors (DRD2) as the historical target of research [7]. Previous animal studies show that chronic consumption of alcohol reduces the expression of DRD2s [8, 9]. In addition, some human neuroimaging studies, but not all [10, 11], have shown reduced DRD2 density in AUD subjects compared to healthy controls [12,13,14] which has been associated with craving and relapse [11, 15]. Though there is some controversy [16], this line of research provides evidence in support of DRD2 involvement in AUD related processes.

In the last 20 years, the dopamine D3 receptor (DRD3) has gained research attention because of its highly localized expression along the mesolimbic dopamine pathway [17, 18] and its distinct drug-related function [19, 20]. In contrast to DRD2, some, but not all [21], preclinical studies have observed an upregulation in DRD3s in response to repeated exposure to cocaine [19] and nicotine [20]. These findings were corroborated in humans with studies showing increased DRD3 levels in the post-mortem brains of cocaine overdose subjects [22,23,24] as well as in in vivo investigations of methamphetamine [25] and polystimulant users [26]. Together, these findings suggest a potentially important role of DRD3s in drug-related processes with both preclinical and clinical investigations supporting upregulation of DRD3s in response to chronic drug exposure under some conditions.

Similarly, preclinical studies have observed increased DRD3 expression in rodents following alcohol consumption [27, 28]. Furthermore, blockade of DRD3s using selective pharmacological antagonist (e.g., SB-277011-A) suggest an important role of DRD3 in influencing AUD phenotypes in animal models. For example, studies have shown that treatment with SB-277011-A reduced oral alcohol self-administration in rodents [29,30,31]. Additionally, compared to first-line approved AUD medications naltrexone and acamprosate [4], SB-277011-A reduced reinstatement of alcohol seeking (i.e., animal model of craving or relapse-like behaviour) in mice [30]. This finding was validated by another study showing a dose-dependent reduction of alcohol seeking reinstatement by SB-277011-A treatment in rats [27]. This line of research suggests an increase in DRD3 levels under conditions of repeated alcohol use, and a positive correlation between DRD3 levels or functioning and AUD phenotypes such as alcohol reinforcement and craving. In humans, the use of GSK598,809, a DRD3 antagonist, has shown to transiently alleviate nicotine craving in smokers [32], normalize the ventral striatal reward response in some AUD subjects [33], and reduce both appetitive responses and attentional bias to food cues in overweight individuals [34, 35]. Despite some promising clinical results regarding the utility of DRD3 antagonism as pharmacotherapy in addictions, the preclinical findings have not been conclusively translated in human subjects with substance use disorders, therefore more research is needed.

Positron emission tomography (PET) studies allow for the in vivo investigation of neurotransmitter activity and receptor density [36]. However, exploration of DRD3s in humans have been limited due to the scarcity of DRD3 selective radioligands. With the advent of [11C]-(+)-PHNO, a DRD3 preferring agonist with 25- to 48-fold selectivity for DRD3 over DRD2 [37], it is now possible to explore DRD3 densities in vivo. Studies exploring the fraction of [11C]-(+)-PHNO signal that reflect DRD3 levels observe the following percentages in healthy male participants: hypothalamus, 90–100%; substantia nigra (SN) 87–100%; ventral pallidum (VP), 71-75%; globus pallidus (GP), 65-66%; limbic striatum (LST) 26-39%, caudate (CD) 2-21%, putamen (PT) 0-14% [38, 39].

One PET study has employed the use of [11C]-(+)-PHNO and a selective DRD3 antagonist, GSK598809, to examine DRD3 levels in AUD subjects compared to healthy controls [40]. This study found no difference in baseline [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding, which reflects receptor levels, between AUD subjects and healthy control in all brain regions apart from the hypothalamus (90–100% of [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding signal in the hypothalamus is attributable to DRD3s [38, 39]). These findings suggested a lack of global DRD3 upregulation in AUD subjects, contrary to predictions [40]. However, their AUD population had wide variation in abstinence periods with a mean (± standard deviation [SD]) of 415 ± 254 (range: 39–893) abstinent days [40]. Therefore, it is currently unknown whether the lack of global DRD3 upregulation (except for in the hypothalamus) observed in the study is due to a recovery of DRD3 levels or a true lack of chronic alcohol induced DRD3 upregulation. Furthermore, no study has investigated the relationship between DRD3 receptor binding and human laboratory measures of AUD phenotypes such as alcohol craving and self-administration.

The current study examined the regulation of DRD3s using PET [11C]-(+)-PHNO in early abstinent AUD subjects compared to healthy controls. Based on the preclinical research discussed above, we hypothesized increased [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in AUD subjects compared to healthy controls, specifically in DRD3-rich areas (i.e. SN, VP, GP). In addition, based on previous human neuroimaging studies, we expect to see decreased [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in DRD2-rich areas of interest (associate striatum [AST], sensorimotor striatum [SMST]) as well as in DRD2/3 mixed areas (LST). This study also explores the relationship between DRD3 levels in AUD subjects and motivation to consume alcohol, measured by an intravenous self-administration paradigm, as well as alcohol craving, measured by a cue-reactivity procedure. In line with previous research [27, 29,30,31], we hypothesize that higher DRD3 levels among AUD subjects will be associated with increased alcohol motivation and craving. Finally, in an exploratory manner, we examine the relationship between [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding and various self-report measures of AUD phenotypes, including alcohol demand (i.e., the relative reinforcing value of alcohol).

Method

Participants

AUD participants

AUD subjects were non-treatment seekers recruited from the community in Toronto, Ontario, and invited for in-person assessments at the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) following a phone pre-screen. At the in-person eligibility assessment, consent was obtained before continuing with study procedures. All AUD participants were assessed for DSM-5 AUD criteria using a modified version of the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID) [41]. Demographic (e.g., age, gender, race, ethnicity) and drug use information were collected. AUD participants provided biological samples, including urine and blood for analysis of drug use and general health, as well as breath samples for breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) and carbon monoxide (CO) measurements for verification of smoking status (threshold for smoking status was 10 ppm). Eligible participants met DSM-5 criteria for AUD, had a negative urine drug screen (which tested for current use of all major illicit drugs and marijuana), and could abstain from drugs and alcohol prior to study visits. Exclusion criteria included other substance use disorders (SUDs) or other disorders, (e.g., mood and anxiety disorders, psychosis, etc.), history of seizures or suicide ideation, pregnancy, treatment seeking for AUD, and conditions or treatments that were contraindications for study procedures (e.g., medication for which alcohol is contraindicated and psychotropic medication). AUD participants that were deemed eligible were enroled in the study and invited to complete the following study procedures. All procedures were approved by the CAMH Research Ethics Board and were conducted according to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. Eligible AUD participants were scheduled for the various study sessions (e.g., PET scan, cue reactivity, and IVASA). Sessions did not occur in a particular order and most sessions occurred on separate days. In some cases, the PET scan and cue-reactivity session occurred on the same day. For these cases, the PET scan occurred first in order to avoid cue effects on radioligand binding.

Control participants

Healthy controls (HCs) were selected out of previously recruited pool of healthy subjects that were scanned with [11C]-(+)-PHNO (recruitment criteria for these participants have been described elsewhere [42, 43]). HCs were chosen to be matched on age, sex, ethnic background, and smoking status with the AUD population.

PET imaging

Abstinence

Participants underwent a single PET scan during early abstinence, where they were instructed to abstain from drinking for 2–7 days and abstain from smoking tobacco overnight prior to scanning. Participants attended an in-person visit during the abstinence period to verify alcohol abstinence by self-report and BrAC readings (0.0 mg%); these measures were collected again immediately prior to scanning. Overnight abstinence from tobacco smoking was verified prior to scan using CO measurements (i.e., below 10 ppm). All withdrawal symptoms were monitored and managed by medical staff according to the guidelines in the supplementary material. One participant needed the engagement of the medical management of withdrawal protocol (Supplementary Material; Medical Management Protocol).

Image acquisition

The PET scan was performed using a high-resolution head-dedicated PET camera system (CTI PET Systems-High Resolution Research Tomography (HRRT), Siemens Medical Imaging, Knoxville, TN). This process has been described elsewhere [44]. The synthesis of [11C]-(+)-PHNO has also been describe elsewhere [45]. Participants underwent a standard proton density weighted brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) on a Discovery MR750 3 T MRI scanner (General Electric, 3 T MR750) to aid region of interest delineation of PET images.

Regions of interest (ROI)

The ROIs consisted of functional striatal areas (LST, AST, and SMST) and DRD3-rich extra striatal areas (SN, VP, GP). The delineation process has been previously described [42]. Outcome measures were ROI non-displaceable binding potentials (BPND) which were estimated using the simplified reference tissues model (SRTM) [46] and a cerebellar cortex (excluding vermis) reference region.

Cue reactivity

The cue exposure paradigm was modified from a previously validated procedure [47]. During this procedure, participants were seated in a room with instruction provided through a computer and they were observed through a 2-way mirror. The session began with a 3-min relaxation period where participants were acclimated to the environment. After this, participants underwent 4 cue exposure trials. Each trial consisted of the introduction of a cup containing a beverage as well as the associated commercially labelled bottle under a cover, so the participant remained blinded to the content of the cue until instructed. The cues for the neutral conditions were a glass of water and a water bottle, while the cues for the alcohol condition was the participants preferred alcohol beverage (e.g., wine, beer, hard liquor; previously collected) and associated container. When instructed, participants lifted the cover and interacted with the cue. Participants were told to hold and smell the cup when they heard a high pitch tone and place the cup down when they heard a low pitch tone. There were 13 signals to smell the liquid contents in each 3-min trial. There were 2 neutral and 2 alcohol condition trials each separated by 3-min relaxation periods, for a total of 4 cue trials. To avoid carry over effects [48, 49], the neutral cue condition occurred before the alcohol condition for all participants. After each trial and the acclimation period, participants completed the Alcohol Urge Questionnaire (AUQ) [50] and an urge question on an 8 point likert scale (“How strong is your urge to drink alcohol right now?”). Outcome measures were AUQ and urge question scores that were averaged across the 2 individual cue trials for each cue condition.

Intravenous self-administration

The intravenous self-administration (IVASA) procedure used here was consistent with our other studies using an IVASA paradigm [51, 52], but modified to involve a progressive ratio (PR) schedule [53]. Participants were given the opportunity to intravenously infuse alcohol on a PR schedule using the Computer Assisted Infusion system (CAIS)[54], which uses a physiologically-based pharmacokinetic (PBPK) model to calculate infusion rates to achieve equivalent increments in BrAC across subjects. Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol for 24 h prior to the procedure and had to provide a BrAC reading of 0.0 mg% for the procedure to begin. Nursing staff inserted an indwelling catheter, and participants were subsequently seated on a reclining chair in front of a computer. Participants were instructed to push a button to obtain alcohol during the 180-min session. Alcohol was delivered by a CAIS initiated infusion of 6% (v/v) alcohol-in-saline solution that targeted an incremental BrAC increase of 10 mg% over 2.5 min after each completed work set.

Participants began the 180-min session with a 13.5-minute-long priming phase. The priming phase served as a period to familiarize the participants with the infusion process while exposing all participants to 3 infusions (requiring 6 button presses each) aimed at reaching a BrAC of 30 mg%. After this phase, participants were free to choose if they wanted more alcohol, for which they needed to complete additional “work sets” (a total number of button presses), with each alcohol infusion requiring larger work sets (Supplementary Table 1).There was a maximum of 20 earnable infusions (total button presses required = 27,420) throughout the session. When infusions were not in progress, the software specified a descending BrAC rate of −1 mg% per min. There was a safety ceiling of 150 mg%, such that further alcohol delivery was prevented until the subsequent infusion would not cause the participant to exceed this threshold. This procedure allows for more control over BrAC levels while eliminating alcohol associated environmental cues for more precise investigation of motivation to consume alcohol [54].

Upon completion of the session, medical staff disconnected the participant from the infusion pumps and removed the catheter. Participants remained in the lab until their BrAC dropped to below 20 mg%. Outcome measures for this session were peak BrAC, BrAC area under the curve (AUC), completed work sets, and cumulative button presses.

Self-report measures

Throughout the study, participants completed a battery questionnaire. Alcohol use and dependence severity was measured by the 10-item alcohol use disorder identification test (AUDIT; covers alcohol consumption, drinking behaviour and alcohol-related problems) [55] and the 25-item alcohol dependence scale (ADS; covers loss of behavioural control over alcohol consumption, obsessive-compulsive drinking, and alcohol withdrawal symptoms)[56]. Separate from the cue-reactivity session, alcohol craving was assessed using the 5-item single-factor Penn Alcohol craving scale (PACS; covers craving characteristics, e.g., how often, how long, how strong)[57]. Scores for each measure were calculated using validated methods.

The relative reinforcing effect of alcohol was examined using the alcohol purchase task (APT) [58, 59]. The APT was collected as part of battery of questionnaires mentioned above. Five outcomes derived from the APT were of interest for the present study: intensity, breakpoint, maximum expenditure (Omax), price associated with Omax (Pmax), and alpha (elasticity across the demand curve). As described in a recent methodological review of the APT [60], intensity describes consumption at no cost (amount of alcohol purchased at $0.00). Breakpoint reflects the first price to reduce consumption to 0 (if this point was not reached, breakpoint was assigned a value of the highest cost—$15.00). Omax is calculated as the greatest single expenditure across the demand curve and Pmax was calculated as the price associated with this point of greatest expenditure [50]. Finally, alpha reflects the rate of change in elasticity across the demand curve (fit with an exponentiated function, as described elsewhere [61]). In other words, alpha reflects observed changes in the sensitivity of hypothetical consumption of alcohol as a function of price, with lower values of alpha corresponding to greater demand for alcohol [62]. Finally, alpha reflects elasticity across the demand curve (fit with an exponentiated function, as described elsewhere [61]).

Data analysis

[11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND values were analyzed using a group x ROI multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) with group (AUD, HC) as independent variables while ROIs (LST, AST, SMST, SN, GP, VP) were dependent variables. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs where conducted to investigate region-specific group differences in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND. Bivariate correlation analyses were conducted to investigate the relationship between [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in AUD participants alone and the various clinical (demographic, questionnaire) as well as laboratory measures (craving, IVASA measures).

In the cue-reactivity paradigm, differences scores of all outcomes (AUQ and urge question) were calculated for both the alcohol and neutral condition (i.e., condition minus baseline). Conditions were compared to each other using t tests. The delta between alcohol and neutral difference scores were used in the correlation analysis to explore relationship between cue-elicited craving and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND.

Prior to analysis the APT data were cleaned according to a 3-criterion algorithm proposed by Stein and colleagues [63]. This algorithm detects cases that violate the assumptions of trend (a global non-negligible reduction in consumption from the first to last price), bounce (less than 10% price-to-price increases in consumption), and reversals from zero (consumption subsequent to a zero-value consumption at a given lower price) [52]. Of the 16 participants that completed this questionnaire (note the varying # of subjects in different analysis in the “AUD subjects” section of the results), 14 subjects were retained for this analysis: one case was removed for violating the trend criterion (invariant consumption across all prices) and one case for violating the reversal from zero criterion. The primary outcomes derived from the APT data, as noted above, were intensity, breakpoint, Omax, Pmax, and alpha (where lower levels of alpha reflect higher demand for alcohol [62]). These outcomes were derived using the beezdemand R package [62]. Equations were fit using the exponentiated approach described elsewhere [61].

Bonferroni’s corrections were applied on all multiple comparisons which were applicable in all ROI analysis (corrected significance level p = 0.0083). For all other analysis (e.g., validation data) results were considered significant at p < 0.05 (SPSS ver. 25.0).

Results

Participants

A total of 17 AUD subjects were scanned and compared to 18 HCs. These groups were matched on age (AUD: 42.35 ± 12.39, HC: 40.72 ± 10.68), sex (AUD: 76.5% male, HC: 77.8% male), smoking status (AUD: 30% smokers, HC: 39% smokers) and race (AUD and HC: 100% white). Chi-square analysis and independent samples t tests showed that HCs did not differ from AUD subjects in these measures. Alcohol related characteristics and PET scan parameters for the AUD sample are described in Table 1. AUD subjects met an average of 4.12 ± 1.73 DSM-5 AUD criteria (i.e., moderate AUD). Of note, there were different sample sizes for different analyses below due to external complications with the study (e.g., COVID-19) which resulted in some participants not completing certain study procedures (e.g., IVASA, questionnaires, etc.). Therefore, there were 35 total subjects in the [11C]-(+)-PHNO group analysis (17 AUD [all 17 also used in clinical correlations], 18 HCs), 16 AUD participants included in the cue-reactivity analysis, and 9 AUD subjects in the IVASA analysis.

Table 1 This table describes AUD related characteristics and PET scan parameters for the AUD group.

[11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND (n = 35)

One-way group (2 levels) x ROI (6 levels) MANOVA revealed a significant group difference when ROIs were jointly considered (Wilk’s Λ = .58, F(6,28) =3.33, p = 0.013, η2p = 0.42; Fig. 1). As Box’s M (47.63) was not significant (p = 0.013) at an alpha level of p = 0.001 [64], the assumption of homogeneity of covariance is met. While binding levels in the AUD group were lower than in HCs in most ROIs (SN, VP, GP, AST, SMST; percent difference range: −1.6 to −16.1%), they were higher in the LST (4.0%; Table 2). This suggests that AUD subjects demonstrated lower [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND than HCs when looking at the examined ROIs generally. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs were conducted for each ROI with a Bonferroni adjusted significance level of p = 0.0083 to account for multiple comparisons. These univariate tests revealed that while [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the SMST was lower in AUD subjects (M ± SD; 2.11 ± 0.47) compared to HCs (2.41 ± 0.35), this did not reach the Bonferroni adjusted significance level (F(1,33) =4.49, p = 0.042, η2p = 0.12). There were no group difference in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in any other ROI. These pattern of results were maintained when age and sex were included as covariates in secondary analysis (MANCOVA: Wilk’s Λ = .57, F(6,26) =3.36, p = 0.016, η2p = 0.43).

Fig. 1: This figure compares the [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND of AUD subjects to those of healthy controls.
figure 1

A one-way MANOVA revealed significantly lower binding in AUD subjects compared to healthy controls when looking at all ROIs jointly (Wilk’s Λ = .58, F(6,28) =3.33, p = 0.013, η2p = 0.42). There were no region-specific differences in binding levels between groups. BPND nondisplaceable binding potential, AST associate striatum, SMST sensorimotor striatum.

Table 2 This table describes the [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND values for the AUD and healthy control groups in all ROIs.

For increased comparability to other PET papers, we also conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA (RMANOVA) with ROIs (6 levels) as within-subject factors and group (2 levels) as a between-subject factor. Because the sphericity assumption was violated, Greenhouse-Geisser results are reported here. The results showed no difference between groups (F(1.519,50.116) =0.938, p = 0.375, η2p = 0.028). This further corroborated the finding that DRD3 levels are not elevated in AUD subjects compared to HCs. This pattern of results were maintained when age (covariate) and sex (between-subject variable) were included in the RMANOVA.

Cue-elicited craving (n = 16)

Main effects

Paired sample t test showed that alcohol cues elicited greater craving than neutral cues, observed in both the AUQ (t (15) =6.612, p < 0.001; Fig. 2A) and the urge question (t (15) =5.310, p < 0.001) while controlling for baseline craving.

Fig. 2: This panel of graphs show the main effects of alcohol craving and the correlations between alcohol-cue-induced craving and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND.
figure 2

A compares cue elicited craving in both the neutral and alcohol cue condition, the alcohol cues eliciting greater craving than neutral cues (t (15) =6.612, p < 0.0001). BD graph the correlation analysis between [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in different ROIs and alcohol cue-elicited craving. These ROIs were chosen to depict DRD3-rich (SN), DRD2-rich (SMST) and DRD2/3 mixed areas (LST). There were no statistically significant correlations except for in the LST which showed a significant positive correlation with binding levels (r = 0.513, p = 0.042), though this did not survive Bonferroni corrections (corrected significance level p = 0.0083). AUQ Alcohol Urge Questionnaire. ***= p value < 0.001.

Correlations

There was a positive correlation between AUQ measured alcohol (minus neutral) cue elicited craving and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND levels in the LST (r = 0.513, p = 0.042), however this did not withstand Bonferroni corrections (corrected significance level p = 0.0083; Fig. 2D). There were no correlations seen with the single urge question.

IVASA (n = 9)

Alcohol consumption

During this session, participants performed an average (±SD) of 5703.22 ± 4611.98 button presses and completed an average of 12 ± 5.05 work sets (i.e., received an average of 12 infusions). This resulted in an average peak BrAC of 64.56 ± 39.19 mg% across all participants at the 60-min (Fig. 3A).

Fig. 3: This panel of graphs shows IVASA alcohol consumption patterns, the correlation between motivation to consume alcohol and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND, and the correlation between [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND and alcohol demand.
figure 3

A shows the time course of breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) throughout the intravenous self-administration session. During this session, participants achieved an average BrAC of 0.65 ± 0.39 mg/dL at around the 60-min mark. BD graph the correlation analysis between [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in different ROIs and peak BrAC achieved during the session. These ROIs were chosen to depict DRD3-rich (SN), DRD2-rich (SMST) and DRD2/3 mixed areas (LST). There were no statistically significant correlations between peak BrAC and binding in any ROI. E This graph depicts the correlation between [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND and the alpha values of the alcohol purchase task (APT). There is a significant positive correlation between these measures such that AUD subjects with lower [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the SN display lower alpha values (r = 0.66, p = 0.0080).

Correlations

There were no correlations observed between IVASA outcome measures (peak BrAC, BrAC AUC, # of work sets, # of button presses) and binding in any ROIs (Fig. 3B–D).

Clinical measures

Correlations

There were no significant correlations between any of the clinical measures analyzed (i.e., daily drink average, # of heavy drinking episodes, PACS, AUDIT, AUD severity, and ADS) and [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND that withstood Bonferroni corrections. In the APT (n = 14), alpha was positively correlated with binding in the SN (r = 0.66, p = 0.0080) after Bonferroni corrections, suggesting that AUD participants with lower [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the SN had lower alpha levels, demonstrating higher demand for alcohol (Fig. 3E).

Discussion

This is the first study using [11C]-(+)-PHNO to show reduced DRD2/3 levels in AUD subjects compared to controls. Specifically, the main finding was a significant group difference in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND between AUD subjects and HCs when looking at examined ROIs generally. An inspection of BPND values showed that AUD subjects had lower binding (2–16%), reflecting lower receptor levels, in 83% of the examined ROIs compared to HCs. That said, analysis of individual ROIs showed no significant group difference in region-specific receptor levels. Contrary to our hypothesis, these results suggest a non-region-specific reduction in DRD2/3 levels in early abstinent AUD subjects. Specifically, we did not observe increased [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in DRD3-rich areas (SN,VP,GP), suggesting that DRD3 are not upregulated in early abstinent AUD subjects. We also did not observe region-specific reductions of [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in DRD2-rich areas (AST, SMST) or DRD2/3 mixed areas (LST). In addition, we did not find an association between binding levels and alcohol cue-induced craving or IVASA measures that withstood corrections. These results suggest that dopamine receptor levels are reduced in AUD subjects in early abstinence but provide no evidence regarding region-specific reductions or associations between receptor levels and our measures of AUD phenotypes. Therefore, these results warrant further investigation.

The primary findings here demonstrate lower [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in AUD compared to HCs in the examined ROIs combined. We did not observe difference in receptor levels when considering DRD3-rich areas (SN,GP, VP), suggesting a lack of DRD3 upregulation in early abstinent AUD subjects. This is inconsistent with previous preclinical research that have demonstrated increased DRD3 expression in the striatum of rats [27] as well as in the dorsal striatum of mice [28] following voluntary alcohol intake. Animal research suggests that chronic alcohol intake may promote increases in DRD3 expression, therefore, one would expect similar trends in AUD subjects, who chronically consume alcohol. Yet, while contrary to preclinical research, our findings are consistent with the previous [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET study that also found a lack of increased DRD3 levels in all AUD brain regions when examining BPND[40]. The previous study did find an elevation of [11 C]-(+)-PHNO volume of distribution in the hypothalamus of subjects with AUD, however, there was no significant elevation while evaluating [11 C]-(+)-PHNO binding potential. Due to the design of our study, we were unable to assess [11C]-(+)-PHNO volume of distribution. In addition, our imaging analysis template did not include hypothalamus delineation. Therefore, we cannot rule out the existence of group differences in DRD3 levels based on the divergent methods used in the current study.

Considering similar findings in both PET studies for comparable ROI, the increased DRD3 expression seen particularly in striatal areas of animal models [27, 28] may not translate to human AUD subjects. Furthermore, our results show that AUD subjects demonstrated a 16.1% decrease in [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in the SN (87–100% reflective of DRD3 levels [38, 39]), suggesting a decrease in DRD3 levels, though this did not reach significance. This is in line with previous studies that have used DRD2/3 radioligands including [11C]FLB 457 [65] and [18F]fallypride [66] that showed non-significantly decreased receptor binding in the SN of AUD subjects compared to healthy controls. Therefore, not only did we fail to observe DRD3 upregulation, but there may be evidence to support a downregulation of DRD3 in early abstinent AUD. The present study adds to the literature by demonstrating that this lack of DRD3 upregulation is unlikely due to a potential recovery of DRD3 levels upon abstinence as our sample was scanned with an average (±SD) of 6.59 ± 4.14 days of abstinence (in contrast to the 415 ± 254 days of abstinence in previous PET study [40]).

On the other hand, our results of reduced [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND when considering all examined ROI together are more in line with previous PET literature investigating DRD2 expression in AUD. PET studies using antagonist radiotracers (e.g., [11C]raclopride, [12, 13, 67, 68] [18F]desmethoxyfallypride [14, 69]) have typically reported lower striatal DRD2/3 levels in AUD subjects compared to healthy controls. While we did not confirm our hypothesis of region-specific reductions in DRD2-rich areas (LST, AST, SMST [39]), we did observe statistically significant global DRD2/3 reductions in AUD subjects with a 12.3% reduction in the SMST (region-specific reduction in SMST did not reach significance). Taken together with the inspection of [11C]-(+)-PHNO BPND in DRD3-rich areas, these results add to the literature by replicating reduced DRD2/3 levels in AUD subjects as seen in previous studies, while also demonstrating that DRD3 levels are not upregulated and may potentially be downregulated.

There is the argument that since DRD2 and DRD3 are colocalized, regulation of one receptor subtype (e.g., DRD3 upregulation) may be masked by the regulation of another in the opposite direction (e.g., DRD2 downregulation). While our study did not address this directly, a previous PET study found that occupancy of a selective DRD3 antagonist (GSK598809) was similar in both AUD and control patients [40], suggesting that the ratio of DRD2/3 in various ROI is unchanged under conditions of chronic alcohol consumption. This makes the argument of compensatory DRD2 reduction alongside DRD3 increases unlikely.

The current study found that alcohol cues elicit significantly greater cravings than neutral cues, and craving increases were positively associated with [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the LST, however this was not significant after Bonferroni corrections. Given that [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the LST is predominated by DRD2 (26-39% reflective of DRD3 levels) [38, 39], and previous human PET studies have reported a negative association between alcohol-cue-induced craving and DRD2 levels [14, 15], these findings seem contradictory. The previous PET studies used different methodologies (e.g., pictorial stimuli for cue-reactivity) and are limited in number, which adds difficulty in interpreting the current findings. Therefore, future studies should aim to illuminate the relative contributions of DRD2 and DRD3 levels in human alcohol craving behaviour.

Most measures from our IVASA paradigm were not associated with [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding. [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in SN (almost exclusively represents DRD3 levels [38, 39]) was positively associated with alpha (lower alpha reflects higher demand for alcohol [62]) in the APT. This suggests that those with lower DRD3 levels in the SN demonstrated higher demand for alcohol. This supports our overall findings of lower DRD2/3 (and specifically a non-significant 16.1% decreased in SN DRD3) levels in AUD subjects compared to HCs and suggests that lower DRD3 may be associated with increased demand for alcohol, which may be a driving factor in the increased alcohol consumption that is characteristic of AUD subjects. The direction of this finding is somewhat unanticipated based on preclinical literature that shows an association between increased DRD3 expression and greater alcohol use [27, 28]. This inconsistency may be due to a difference between the animal and human condition, further suggesting that the relationship between DRD3 and the reinforcing effects of alcohol seen in animals may not translate to humans. Given our sample, future research should aim to replicate this with larger and more severe sample populations.

This is the first study to explore the association between human laboratory measures of AUD phenotypes (alcohol motivation and cue-induced craving) and [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding, yet the results should be considered within the context of the study limitations. First, while our sample size is comparable to other PET studies comparing drug users to health controls [13, 67, 70], we were likely underpowered to detect the expected correlations between AUD phenotypes and receptor levels. Second, our sample may have less severe AUD compared to some other studies. Our sample met an average of 4.12 ± 1.73 DSM-5 AUD criteria (i.e., moderate AUD) and were recruited from the local community. Other PET studies that investigated dopamine receptor regulation in AUD subjects have recruited from outpatient treatment programs and other therapeutic communities [13, 67, 70]. The qualitative difference in AUD characteristics between sample populations may result in different alcohol related behaviours, which may in turn affect receptor levels. Third, subjects were asked to abstain from cigarette smoking overnight and therefore may have been in nicotine withdrawal during the PET scan. Because we did not measure nicotine withdrawal specifically, we cannot rule out the possibility of an effect on [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding. Fourth, it has been shown that factors like impaired control over alcohol and impulsivity may affect alcohol use[71, 72]. The current study did not investigate these factors; therefore it is unclear how those factors may affect our alcohol consumption in the IVASA procedure and the correlations to [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding. Finally, several brain regions afflicted by volume loss have been reported in conditions associated with AUD [73]. Relatedly, the use of a SRTM model here and the potential for group differences in [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the cerebellum (the reference region) presents a potential confound that may contribute to the observed global reductions DRD2/3 levels. It is possible that binding in the cerebellum is negligible however, we cannot rule out the possibility of group differences in [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the reference region. Therefore, it is unknown whether brain tissue volume loss or group differences in cerebellar tissue volume affected receptor levels or [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding values observed here.

In conclusion, this study used PET imaging with [11C]-(+)-PHNO to explore DRD3 regulation and function in AUD subjects. We found that early abstinent AUD subjects had lower non-region-specific [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding, reflecting lower DRD2/3 levels, compared to healthy controls. This replicates findings from a previous PET studies and extends the literature by suggesting a lack of increased DRD3 levels in early abstinence AUD. In addition, while DRD3 levels were not associated with our measures of alcohol motivation, there was an uncorrected positive correlation between DRD2/3 levels in the LST and alcohol cue-induced craving. As this finding did not survive Bonferroni corrections, it warrants further investigation with a larger sample size. Future studies should address the limitations presented here and aim to validate the relationship between DRD3s and AUD phenotypes in humans.